Anthropology

Anthropology
Open Access

ISSN: 2332-0915

Perspective - (2018) Volume 6, Issue 1

Did Hairlessness Stimulate an Increase in Hominin Brain Size? Insights from the Cutaneous Neurosensory Interface and Comparative Vertebrate Morphology

Mitsuhiro Denda1, Gopinathan K Menon2 and Peter M Elias3*
1Shiseido Global Innovation Center, Yokohama, Japan and Japan Science Technology Agency (CREST), Kawaguchi, Japan
2California Academy of Sciences, San Francisco, CA, USA
3Department of Dermatology, VA Medical Center, San Francisco and UC San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA
*Corresponding Author: Peter M Elias, Department of Dermatology, VA Medical Center, 4150 Clement Street, MS 190 San Francisco, CA 94121, USA, Tel: (415) 750-2091, Fax: (415) 750-2106 Email:

Abstract

The basis for the late stages of brain growth in our immediate human ancestors is unknown, but generally attributed to the development of language associated with increasingly complex social settings. We present arguments for an additional or alternative potential contributor to endocranial expansion. As ancestral humans emerged from tropical forests into open savannahs, loss of their body hair facilitated the dissipation of body heat. But their newly-exposed cutaneous surfaces would have been bombarded by exogenous stimuli that could have stimulated an increase in brain size required to centrally process a plethora of new sensory information arriving from the periphery. Two lines of evidence support this hypothesis. First, the common embryologic origin of epidermis and central nervous system (CNS) from the primitive neuroectoderm, endowed the skin with a broad suite of neuro mechanisms that mirrors the CNS, including outward-facing ‘physical’ receptors that monitor changes in visual, acoustic and olfactory stimuli, as well as many neuromediators that impact memory, mood and wakefulness in the CNS. Second, two groups of marine organisms, cephalopods and electric fish, exhibit large brain: body mass ratios associated with heightened cutaneous sensory capabilities. Human nakedness also stimulated new psychosocial and stress responses, mediated by cutaneous oxytocin and glucocorticoids, respectively. Together, these insights suggest that the onset of hairlessness could have stimulated an increment of endocranial expansion, shortly before the emergence of modern humans.

Keywords: Brain; Epidermis; Evolution; Hairlessness; Neuroreceptors; Neurosensors; Skin; TRPVs

Abbreviations

CNS: Central Nervous System; GABA: γ-Amino- Butyric Acid; mya: Million Years Ago; O2: Oxygen; TRPV: Transient Receptor Potential Vanilloid

Introduction

While australopithecines exhibited endocranial volumes that were comparable to their great ape ancestors [1], brain volumes began to expand ≈3 million years ago (mya) with the transition from a predominantly vegetarian, arboreal to an increasingly omnivorous, ambulatory lifestyle [2]. Because these new sources of protein were widely dispersed in tropical forests, these ancestral humans established centralized campsites where groups of hunters developed new weapons and food processing tools, as they ventured further afield. Together with the increased stress of group living, which placed a premium on intragroup cooperation, these forces together selected for a further expansion in brain volume [3]. The next increment of brain growth began 1-1.5 mya as ancestral hominins (H. erectus) aggregated at campsites, centered around fires [4,5]. Under such crowded circumstances, interactions required the acquisition of cooperative social skills, reasoning, and perhaps the development of proto-languages [6,7], but competition and conflict likely also flourished [8]. Together, these forces likely selected for further brain growth, made possible by cooking of dietary staples, which likely increased the bioavailability of additional nutrients needed to accomplish brain expansion [9]. Thus, the social brain hypothesis (SBH) argues that large brain size in primates represent an evolutionary response to complex social interactions and information-rich social environments.

Yet, the basis for last increments of brain growth that occurred shortly before, and perhaps concurrent with the emergence of modern humans [10,11], is speculative. As still-furred early humans ranged further and further from forest cover, they risked overheating as they exerted themselves in open savannahs. It has been suggested that bipedalism reduced the impact of solar radiation fluxes and made possible reduction of body hair (setting them apart from savannah quadrupeds) [12]. But, rapid, widespread dispersion of cutaneous eccrine glands in service to thermoregulation [13] would have been impeded by a dense furry mantle, risking excessive insolation (i.e., heat retention) under the harsh environmental conditions that prevailed in Sub-Saharan Africa ≈1-1.5 mya (e.g., [14]). Hence, eccrine gland proliferation likely selected for a subsequent, dramatic reduction in hominins’ furry pelage, necessitated by the need to dissipate heat aided by not only sweating, but also by insensible, constitutive transepidermal water loss (TEWL) [15]. TEWL provides a further evolutionary advantage by allowing a more efficient conservation of electrolytes. Thus, the high rates of TEWL likely would also have selected for new mutations that improved epidermal permeability barrier function, the dominant imperative for life in a xeric terrestrial environment [15-17].

Insights from the Cutaneous Neurosensory Interface

Common Origin of Epidermis and Central Nervous System: Because newly exposed, naked hominin skin would have suddenly been bombarded by an array of new sensations, the need to integrate this new burden of sensory information could have selected for a further increment in brain growth. This hypothesis is anchored in the common origin of the central nervous system (CNS) and epidermis from the layer of embryonic neuroectoderm that coats the developing embryo. As the CNS buds off the neuroectodermal layer, this layer, which gave rise to the epidermis, remains endowed with a full repertoire of the same neuro mediators and neuroreceptors that are present in the CNS.

The Neurosensory Consequences of Hair Loss: As noted above, a central pillar of this hypothesis; i.e., that shedding of hair could have stimulated further brain enlargement, relates to the common embryological origin of the epidermis and the CNS, resulting in the epidermis’ retention of a full suite of neuro mechanisms that are traditionally associated with nervous tissues (Tables 1 and 2). While the roles of several of these neuroreceptors in the skin have been identified, many are still ‘orphan receptors,’ because their functions in epidermis remain incompletely characterized. Nonetheless, most persisted, and the ones that have been characterized clearly provide the skin with a remarkable panoply of sensory capabilities. These include not only a broad suite of ‘physical receptors’ that monitor the external environment for changes in osmotic, visual, auditory, mechanical, temperature and olfactory stimuli [18] (Table 1), but also a plethora of neuro mediators associated with ‘higher’ functions, including emotional responses, memory and seizure activity (Table 2). This plethora of cutaneous neurosensors would have remained relatively dormant, while hominin skin remained coated with a dense pelage, but the wide-spread loss of body hair would have exposed the newly hairless epidermis of our immediate hominin ancestors to a sudden onslaught of sensory stimuli [19,20]. Although other mammals, like some primates and elephants, display comparable brain:body mass ratios to ours, the number of neurons in human brains exceeds that of any other vertebrates [21]. Thus, an urgent need for central processing of this sudden sensory bombardment from the periphery could have stimulated a late increase in hominin brain size (Figure 1).

Transient Receptor(TRPV)Family Role References
TRPV 1, 3 4 Osmotic pressure pH (<6.6),
Temperature (>35-43° c),
Barrier homeostasis
Liedtke W. 2007 Exp physiol. 92: 507-512
Denda M. 2011 Adv Exp Med Biol.704:847-860
Denda M. 2007 J Invest Dermatol 127: 654-659
TRPA-1 Temperature (<17°), pain,
mechanical stress,
Barrier homeostasis
Talavera k. 2009 Nat Neurosci 12: 1293-1299
Denda M. 2010 J invest Dermatol 130: 1942-1945
TRPM-8 Temperature (<22°C),
Barrier homeostasis
Tsutsumi M. 2010 J invest Dermatol 130: 1945-1948
Denda M. 2010 Exp Derm 19(9) 791-795s
Visual light receptors
Rhodopsin, Opsins Barrier homeostasis Tsutsumi M. 2009 Exp Derm 18(6) 567-570
Melatonin Barrier homeostasis Denda M. 2005 Dry skin and moisturizers, 2nd Edition 155-160
Acoustic
High - Frequency sound
(20-100Khz)
Hypersonic effects Barrier homeostasis Kawai N. 2001 Neuro reports 12: 3419-3423
Oohashi T. 2006 Brain Res 1073-4: 339-347
Denda M. 2005 Br J Dermatol 162: 503-507
Olfactory
OR2AT4 Wounding healing, epidermal
Proliferation
Busse D. 2014 J Invest Dermatol 134(11) 2823-2832

Table 1: Physical receptors at the cutaneous- environmental interface.

Neurotransmitters Role Partial List of References
Histamine   Inflammation, Itch, Barrier homeostasis Yamamaura K, J toxicol sci 34(4) 427-431, 2009
Ashida Y, JID 116(2) 261-265, 2001
Giustizieri ML, JACI 114(5) 1176-1182, 2004
Dopamine Barrier homeostasis +other? Fuziwara S, JID 125(4) 783-789, 2005
Serotonin - Denda M, 2005 Skin Moisturizer, 2nd edition 155-160
Nicotinic   Antimicrobial Défense, Barrier homeostasis Radek K, Cell host micro 27(7) 277-289, 2010
Denda M. JID 121(2) 362-367, 2003
GABA Barrier homeostasis+ keratinocyte proliferation Denda M, JID 119(5) 1041-1047, 2002
Glycerine Barrier homeostasis+ keratinocyte proliferation Denda M, JID 121 (2) 362-367, 2003
Aldrenergic Barrier homeostasis+ keratinocyte proliferation Denda M, JID 121 (1) 142-148, 2003
Glutamate Barrier homeostasis+ keratinocyte proliferation Fuziwara S, JID 120(6) 1023-1029, 2003
Adenosine Barrier homeostasis+ keratinocyte proliferation Denda M, JID 119(5) 1034-1040, 2002
Neurohormones    
cannabinoids Barrier homeostasis, Inflammation Roelandt T, Exp Dermatol 21(9) 688-693, 2012
opioids Inflammation, Itch Bigliardi PL, Exp Derm 18(5) 424-430, 2009
CRF, ACTH, GC Stress responses Slominski A, Physiol Rev 80(3) 979-1020, 2000

Table 2: Other neuro mediators at the cutaneous-environmental interface.

anthropology-Endo-cranial

Figure 1: Endo cranial volumes plotted against time, showing an accelerated change in volume from homo erectus to anatomically modern homo in the last Pleistocene (modified from (3)).

Epidermal ‘Physical Receptors’: Transient receptor potential vanilloids (TRPV) sense changes in temperature, among other physical and chemical factors in both the epidermis [22] and CNS [23]. TRPVs are activated in response to changes in skin surface pressure, temperature, acidity and osmolarity, which in turn regulate epidermal permeability barrier homeostasis [24-26]. But visible light exposure also influences epidermal permeability barrier homeostasis [27], and certain proteins in keratinocytes, including rhodopsin, blue-opsin, red-green opsin, transducin and phosphodiesterase 6, that play crucial roles in retinal visual perception [28,29]. Although their function in skin is still unclear, exposure to strong light modifies systemic hormone levels, even in humans afflicted with congenitally impaired vision [30]. Moreover, the epidermis synthesizes melatonin, a classic light-sensitive hormone as well as abundantly expressing melatonin receptors [31]. Therefore, the skin acts as a peripheral sensor of visual signals en route to processing in the brain.

Likewise, sound, via still unidentified, cutaneous auditory receptor(s), also influences whole-body physiology and epidermal homeostasis. For example, live music (at sound frequencies >20 kHz) influences performers’ brain waves and hormone levels, but the acoustic stimuli have little impact when music of this frequency derives from compact disk sources [32,33]. Yet, when a sound barrier separates test subjects from the live music source, the impact of live music disappears (Ibid.), demonstrating a putative role for peripheral auditory processing. Pertinently, puffs of air applied to the skin surface influence human auditory perception, and even inaudible bursts of air applied to the hands or neck result in mishearing ‘b’ as ‘p’ [34]. Finally, exposure of hairless mouse skin to auditory frequencies of 10-30 kHz significantly enhances permeability barrier homeostasis [23,35]. Thus, the exposed skin of humans recognizes and processes small bursts of sound via cutaneous auditory receptors, but whether these responses require prior central processing is not known. In summary, detection of high-frequency sounds by exposed, hairless skin might have provided a further stimulus for brain growth as well as a survival advantage for ancestral, newly-hairless hominins threatened by large predators or natural disasters.

It should also be noted that keratinocytes express a plethora of (>20) olfactory receptors whose role in normal skin physiology remains largely unexplored. But activation of one of these orphan receptors, OR2AT4, by a sandalwood odorant, stimulates wound healing, while promoting proliferation and migration of human keratinocytes [36]. Once again, whether these epidermal responses require prior central processing is not yet known.

Additional Epidermal Neuromechanisms- the ‘Brain in the Skin’: Beyond the ‘physical neuroreceptors,’ human epidermis also deploys an array of additional neurotransmitters and receptors (Table 2), including dopamine, serotonin, histamine and γ-amino-butyric acid (GABA), whose activation traditionally is associated with regulating higher CNS functions, such as memory, emotion, seizure activity and wakefulness. Moreover, epidermis also elaborates (and receives) signals from a suite of neurohormones, including opioids, cannabinoids, adrenocorticoids, and oxytocin [37]. The last mediates important social cues, including the establishment of intimacy and the ability to empathize, both of which are crucial for interpersonal bonding [38,39]. Clearly, stimulation of these peripheral neuroreceptors must stimulate central processing. Finally, psychosocial studies suggest that cutaneous stimuli, facilitated by the loss of a dense pelage, influence human emotion [40]. Light physical contact, tends to increase generosity [41,42], and could have been critical for social communication as language systems developed [6,43]. Thus, naked skin not only enabled us to interact more efficiently with our physical environment, but it also plays an important role in social interactions and psychological well-being [44].

Discussion

Relevant examples from other organisms

While the sudden, widespread shedding of a dense furry mantle would have uncovered newly exposed, epidermal neuroreceptors to continuous bombardment by often urgent peripheral sensations, there are other examples in nature of ‘naked skin’ associated with unusual brain size or complexity. The octopus provides pertinent evidence that a plethora of peripheral sensations can stimulate brain growth [45]. Because of its radial symmetry, the octopus must integrate sensory inputs not only from its eight, separate peripheral ‘brains,’ but also from its highly ‘sentient’ skin, and enormous, human-like eyes. Likely as a result, the octopus evolved a walnut-sized brain with 6-fold more neurons than occur in rodents [46,47]. Not only is the octopus intellect considered comparable to canines, perhaps exceeding even the most sentient of avians [48]. The combination of a peripheral and central sensory system has further endowed the octopus with a suite of ‘humanlike’ characteristics, including a capacity to learn, use tools, distinguish between individuals, playfulness that can become malevolent, and perhaps even human-like, emotional responses [49]. Cutaneous visual mechanisms, with which cephalopods are richly endowed, allow rapid camouflage responses in a wide variety of habitats [50].

An even more dramatic example of brain expansion in response to peripheral stimuli is the electric fish of the genus Gnathonemus. These fish possess electroreceptors in their epidermis that allow organisms to accurately assess the distance, size and shape of surrounding objects by transmitting an ‘electric image’ to the brain [51]. As a result, the brain:body mass ratio of these fish is higher than any known vertebrate, including humans, also accounting for 60% of bodily oxygen (O2) consumption, compared to the 2%-8% of O2 utilization in resting humans [52]. The hypertrophy of Gnathonemus’ brains in response to peripheral electroreceptors provides further support for the concept that the cutaneous boundary layer could influence brain size.

Another group of mammals with naked skin, Cetaceans, that occupy a marine environment and display hierarchical social structures, are considered ‘intelligent’ and show an astonishing similarity to humans and other primates in social, behavioral and neuroanatomical complexity [53]. Fox et al. provides a compelling view of the social and cultural roots of encephalization in whales and dolphins. These creatures “show overwhelming evidence for sophisticated social and prosocial behavior, including complex alliance relationships, social transfer of hunting techniques, cooperative hunting, complex vocalizations including regional group dialects, vocal mimicry and signature whistles unique to individuals, interspecific cooperation with humans, allopreening and social play.” These characteristics all rely on a highly-developed sense of touch (Ibid.). Cetacean epidermis, like human skin, displays several unique adaptations for aquatic life, including a new, complete absence of pilosebaceous units (i.e., hair) [15]. Thus, a late increment of brain growth in ancestral humans could have occurred in response to the evolution of hairlessness.

Conclusion

It is our hypothesis that loss of hair, with exposure of the epidermis created a new boundary layer that receives and responds to multiple external sensations and signals, as well as social interactions based on changes in appearance and emotional cues (blushing, blanching, touch, pheromones). These peripheral stimuli could have placed additional stress on the CNS, leading to further encephalization, a concept supported by several analogous examples in other vertebrate and invertebrate organisms.

Declaration

Ethics approval and consent to participate: N/A

Consent for publication: N/A

Availability of data and material: Printed/identified publications

Competing Interests: None

Funding

Research reported in this publication was supported by the National Institute of Arthritis, Musculoskeletal and Skin Diseases of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number AR057752, administered by the Northern California Institute for Research and Education, with additional resources provided by a Veterans Affairs Medical Center Merit Review, San Francisco, CA.

Authors Contributions

MD: Wrote initial draft, and provided initial concept

PME: Edited manuscript, and added pertinent references

GM: Provided critical information about evolutionary aspects of hair in mammals.

Acknowledgments

Ms. Joan Wakefield provided invaluable editorial assistance. This content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of either the National Institutes of Health or the Department of Veterans Affairs.

References

  1. Bookstein F, Schafer K, Prossinger H, Seidler H, Fieder M, et al. (1999) Comparing frontal cranial profiles in archaic and modern homo by morphometric analysis. Anat Rec 257: 217-224.
  2. Semendeferi K, Damasio H, Frank R, Van Hoesen GW (1997) The evolution of the frontal lobes: a volumetric analysis based on three-dimensional reconstructions of magnetic resonance scans of human and ape brains. J Hum Evol 32: 375-388.
  3. Holloway RL (2015) Brain evolution. In: Basics in Human Evolution. Elsevier, Amsterdam. pp. 235-250.
  4. Ash J, Gallup GG (2007) Paleoclimatic variation and brain expansion during human evolution. Hum Nat 18: 109-124.
  5. Roberts RG, Bird MI (2012) Evolutionary anthropology: Homo 'incendius'. Nature 485: 586-587.
  6. Holloway RL (2012) Language and tool making are similar cognitive processes. Behav Brain Sciences 35: 326.
  7. Deacon T (1997) The symbolic species: the co-evolution of language and the brain. WW Norton, New York, USA.
  8. Wilson EO (2014) The meaning of human existence. Liveright publishing, New York, USA.
  9. Wrangham R,Carmody R (2010) Human adaptation to the control of fire. EvolAnthropol 19: 187-199.
  10. Hublin JJ, Ben-Ncer A, Bailey SE, Freidline SE, Neubauer S, et al. (2017) New fossils from Jebel Irhoud Morocco and the pan-African origin of Homo sapiens. Nature 546: 289-292.
  11. Richter D, Grun R, Joannes-Boyau R, Steele TE, Amani F, et al. (2017) The age of the hominin fossils from Jebel Irhoud, Morocco, and the origins of the Middle Stone Age. Nature 546: 293-296.
  12. Wheeler PE (1991) The thermoregulatory advantages of hominid bipedalism in open equatorial environments: the contribution of increased convective heat loss and cutaneous evaporative cooling. J Hum Evol 21: 107-115.
  13. Jablonski NG (2006) Skin: a natural history. University of California Press, Berkeley, USA.
  14. DeMenocal PB (2004) African climate change and faunal evolution during the Pliocene-Pleistocene. Earth Planet Sci Lett 220: 3-24.
  15. Elias PM, Williams ML (2016) Basis for the gain and subsequent dilution of epidermal pigmentation during human evolution: The barrier and metabolic conservation hypotheses revisited. Am J Phys Anthropol 161: 189-207.
  16. Elias PM, Williams ML (2013) Re-appraisal of current theories for the development and loss of epidermal pigmentation in hominins and modern humans. J Hum Evol 64: 687-692.
  17. Elias PM, Feingold K (2006) Stratum corneum barrier function: definitions and broad concepts. In: Skin Barrier. Taylor & Francis, New York, USA. pp. 1-4.
  18. Denda M, Nakatani M, Ikeyama K, Tsutsumi M, Denda S (2007) Epidermal keratinocytes as the forefront of the sensory system. Exp Dermatol 16: 157-161.
  19. Lee SH, Wolpoff MH (2003) The pattern of evolution in Pleistocene human brain size. Paleobiology 29: 186-196.
  20. Bruner E (2007) Cranial shape and size variation in human evolution: structural and functional perspectives. Childs NervSyst 23: 1357-1365.
  21. Herculano-Houzel S (2009) The human brain in numbers: a linearly scaled-up primate brain. Front Hum Neurosci 3: 31.
  22. Dhaka A, Viswanath V,Patapoutian A (2006) Trp ion channels and temperature sensation. Annu Rev Neurosci 29: 135-61.
  23. Denda M, Tsutsumi M (2011) Roles of transient receptor potential proteins (TRPs) in epidermal keratinocytes. Adv Exp Med Biol 704: 847-860.
  24. Denda M, Sokabe T, Fukumi-Tominaga T, Tominaga M (2007) Effects of skin surface temperature on epidermal permeability barrier homeostasis. J Invest Dermatol 127: 654-659.
  25. Denda M, Tsutsumi M, Denda S (2010) Topical application of TRPM8 agonists accelerates skin permeability barrier recovery and reduces epidermal proliferation induced by barrier insult: role of cold-sensitive TRP receptors in epidermal permeability barrier homoeostasis. Exp Dermatol 19: 791-795.
  26. Pang Z, Sakamoto T, Tiwari V, Kim YS, Yang F, et al. (2015) Selective keratinocyte stimulation is sufficient to evoke nociception in mice. Pain 156: 656-665.
  27. Denda M,Fuziwara S (2008) Visible radiation affects epidermal permeability barrier recovery: selective effects of red and blue light. J Invest Dermatol 128: 1335-1336.
  28. Tsutsumi M, Ikeyama K, Denda S, Nakanishi J, Fuziwara S, et al. (2009) Expressions of rod and cone photoreceptor-like proteins in human epidermis. Exp Dermatol 18: 567-70.
  29. Goto M, Ikeyama K, Tsutsumi M, Denda S,Denda M (2011) Phosphodiesterase inhibitors block the acceleration of skin permeability barrier repair by red light. Exp Dermatol 20: 568-571.
  30. Czeisler CA, Shanahan TL, Klerman EB, Martens H, Brotman DJ, et al. (1995) Suppression of melatonin secretion in some blind patients by exposure to bright light. N Engl J Med 332: 6-11.
  31. Slominski AT, Kleszczynski K, Semak I, Janjetovic Z, Zmijewski MA, et al. (2014) Local melatoninergic system as the protector of skin integrity. Int J Mol Sci 15: 17705-17732.
  32. Kawai N, Honda M, Nakamura S, Samatra P, Sukardika K, et al. (2001) Catecholamines and opioid peptides increase in plasma in humans during possession trances. Neuroreport 12: 3419-3423.
  33. Oohashi T, Kawai N, Nishina E, Honda M, Yagi R, et al. (2006) The role of biological system other than auditory air-conduction in the emergence of the hypersonic effect. Brain Res 1073-1074: 339-347.
  34. Gick B,Derrick D (2009) Aero-tactile integration in speech perception. Nature 462: 502-504.
  35. Denda M, Nakatani M (2010) Acceleration of permeability barrier recovery by exposure of skin to 10-30 kHz sound. Br J Dermatol 162: 503-507.
  36. Busse D, Kudella P, Gruning NM, Gisselmann G, Stander S, et al. (2014) A synthetic sandalwood odorant induces wound-healing processes in human keratinocytes via the olfactory receptor OR2AT4. J Invest Dermatol 134: 2823-2832.
  37. Slominski AT, Zmijewski MA, Skobowiat C, Zbytek B, Slominski RM, et al. (2012) Sensing the environment: regulation of local and global homeostasis by the skin's neuroendocrine system. Springer-Verlag Frankfurt.
  38. Campbell A (2010) Oxytocin and human social behavior. PersSocPsychol Rev 14: 281-295.
  39. Keri S, Kiss I (2011) Oxytocin response in a trust game and habituation of arousal. PhysiolBehav 102: 221-224.
  40. Ackerman JM, Nocera CC,Bargh JA (2010) Incidental haptic sensations influence social judgments and decisions. Science 328: 1712-1715.
  41. Fisher JD, Rytting M, Heslin R (1976) Hands touching hands: affective and evaluative effects of an interpersonal touch. Sociometry 39: 416-421.
  42. Crusco AH, Wetzel CG (1984) The Midas touch: the effects of interpersonal touch on restaurant tipping. PersSocPsychol Bull 10: 512-517.
  43. Vonk J, Aradhye C (2015) Evolution of Cognition. In: Basics in Human Evolution. Elsevier, Amsterdam. pp. 479-491.
  44. Montagu A (1986) Touching: the human significance of the skin. William Morrow, New York, USA.
  45. Godfrey-Smith P (2016) Other minds: the octopus, the sea, and the deep origins of consciousness. Farrar, Straus and Giroux, New York, USA.
  46. Hochner B (2012) An embodied view of octopus neurobiology. CurrBiol 22: R887-892.
  47. Hochner B (2013) How nervous systems evolve in relation to their embodiment: what we can learn from octopuses and other molluscs. Brain BehavEvol 82: 19-30.
  48. Zarrella I, Ponte G, Baldascino E, Fiorito G (2015) Learning and memory in Octopus vulgaris: a case of biological plasticity. CurrOpinNeurobiol 35: 74-79.
  49. Montgomery S (2016) The soul of an octopus. Atria Books, New York, USA.
  50. Chiao CC, Chubb C, Hanlon RT (2015) A review of visual perception mechanisms that regulate rapid adaptive camouflage in cuttlefish. J Comp PhysiolANeuroethol Sens Neural BehavPhysiol 201: 933-945.
  51. von der Emde G, Schwarz S, Gomez L, Budelli R, Grant K (1998) Electric fish measure distance in the dark. Nature 395: 890-4.
  52. Nilsson G (1996) Brain and body oxygen requirements of Gnathonemuspetersii, a fish with an exceptionally large brain. J ExpBiol 199: 603-7.
  53. Marino L, Connor RC, Fordyce RE, Herman LM, Hof PR, et al. (2007) Cetaceans have complex brains for complex cognition. PLoSBiol 5: e139.
Citation: Denda M, Menon GK, Elias PM (2018) Did Hairlessness Stimulate an Increase in Hominin Brain Size? Insights from the Cutaneous Neurosensory Interface and Comparative Vertebrate Morphology. Anthropol 6: 199.

Copyright: © 2018 Denda M, et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Top