Journal of Depression and Anxiety

Journal of Depression and Anxiety
Open Access

ISSN: 2167-1044

Review Article - (2014) Volume 3, Issue 1

Hedonic Tone: A Bridge between the Psychobiology of Depression and its Comorbidities

Sternat T1,3, Lodzinski A2 and Katzman MA1,2,4,5,6*
1START Clinic for Mood and Anxiety Disorders, Toronto, ON, Canada, E-mail: Sternatt@gmail.com
2Adler Graduate Professional School, Toronto ON, Canada, E-mail: Sternatt@gmail.com
3Janssen Pharmaceuticals Inc, Canada, E-mail: Sternatt@gmail.com
4Department of Psychology, Lakehead University, Thunder Bay, ON, Canada, E-mail: Sternatt@gmail.com
5University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada, E-mail: Sternatt@gmail.com
6The Northern Ontario School of Medicine; Thunder Bay, ON, Canada, E-mail: Sternatt@gmail.com
*Corresponding Author: Katzman MA, START Clinic for Mood and Anxiety Disorders, 32 Park Road, Toronto, Ontario, M4W 2N4, Canada, Tel: 1-416-598-9344, Fax: 1-416-598-8198 Email:

Abstract

Treatment of Major Depressive Disorder (MDD), in many cases, offers less than optimal results, resulting in loss of function and remaining impairment. This is partially explained by the high prevalence of comorbid psychiatric illnesses that share common features, likely resulting in less than adequate treatment outcomes among patients receiving traditional psychiatric treatment. The term anhedonia does not sufficiently capture the complex and multifaceted reward-associated deficits seen in neuropsychiatric disorders. Deficits in reward-related processes can lead to behaviors that may appear to be the loss of interest or pleasure, and may preclude an individual from engaging in goal-oriented action. Investigators have identified a lifelong poor hedonic responsiveness, manifested as altered sensitivity to reinforcement, in patients with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), and suggest it as a common endophenotype for depression and the inattentive symptoms of ADHD. Research into the neurobiology of anhedonia has focused on the centers in the brain related to reward and motivation, and has identified the dopaminergic and associated circuits as playing a key role. Studies have shown that lesions in reward and motivation circuits result in anhedonia, and that decreased activation in the medial frontal cortex is believed to underlie abnormal positive affect processing in patients. In this paper, we propose that a specific phenotype of depression, which is more likely to be described as “treatment resistant” is associated with high risk of comorbidity with dysthymia, ADHD, and substance abuse, specifically cocaine abuse. We believe these patients experience a chronically lower hedonic tone that results in chronic anhedonia (as opposed to acute state anhedonia noted in a major depressive episode) related to unique pathophysiology and requiring treatment of the dopaminergic deficiency that characterizes its unique collection of comorbidities.

Keywords: Anhedonia; Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; Cocaine; Comorbidity; Major depressive disorder, Dopamine; Dopaminergic neurons; Limbic system; Prefrontal cortex; Reward

Introduction

Treatment of Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) in many cases, offers less than optimal results, resulting in loss of function and remaining impairment. In recent years, there has been an accumulation of evidence demonstrating substantial etiopathological heterogeneity and overlap both within and across a variety of psychiatric disorders including depression [1]. While initial research was conducted to gain insight into genetic risk, much of the current research has focused on attempting to develop more effective treatments. This has proven to be difficult, in part, due to the varying degree of etiological heterogeneity of symptoms associated with MDD. Moving beyond genetics, towards an understanding of specific neural pathways and distinct disruptions that may result in related, but different, symptomatic presentation, could lead to a more accurate appraisal and definition of disorders. Still, at present, there is no consensus on how to identify relevant and disease-specific pathology with the specific neural circuits given our current limited understanding of disease pathophysiology.

Hence, the purpose of this paper is to gain insight and to propose a common link between symptoms and related disorders in association with specific dysfunction of known neural circuits, in order to account for the increased likelihood of developing treatmentresistant depression in a subgroup of depressed patients. This paper will specifically attempt to present the question as to whether low hedonic tone is a risk factor for Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and for developing comorbid depression. Furthermore, this paper will present the hypothesis that low hedonic tone and associated specific neurobiologic correlates predict the presence of ADHD in MDD, thus who is at risk for poorer outcomes with selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitors (SSRIs).

Brain Regions and Circuits

Eleven key areas of the brain have been highlighted in part by their specific neurotransmitter projections of which alteration in neurotransmitter innervation has been hypothesized to create disruptions in information processing and consequently specific psychological symptoms [2]. Linking each of these functional areas is a network of neuron projections or neurotransmitter pathways from the brainstem. It is posited that when a neurotransmitter-carrying neuron projects into a specific area, it has the capacity to modulate the physiological processes of the associated circuit [2]. Thus these pathways form the substrates that “tune” the neurons within each circuit, thereby affecting the function of the specific brain areas they innervate. Furthermore, it is hypothesized that these neural circuits are linked together in functional loops allowing various regions of the brain to communicate chemically via neurotransmission. In some instances, for example in cortex-to-cortex circuits, cortical areas are “tuned” by bottom-up neurotransmission, whereas other cortical circuits including a variety of Cortico-Striatal-Thalamic-Cortical Circuits (CSTC), allow information to be sent downstream by way of top-down mediation [3]. Whenever there is malfunction or even hypofunction in these circuits, the effects are felt throughout the loop and thus may mediate specific psychological symptoms.

Neural Circuits and Symptoms of Depression

This model, when applied to depression, illuminates the complex interaction of the three monoaminergic neurotransmitters system comprised of serotonin (5-HT), Norepinephrine (NE), and Dopamine (DA), which act to modulate the information processing across a wide variety of neural circuits, any and all of which may be responsible for mediating various symptoms of depression [3]. Specifically, the basis of this hypothesis rests on the notion that the symptoms of a major depressive episode (MDE) are directly related to inefficient information processing in various neural circuits, which can be mapped topographically to specific brain regions. Stahl [3] posits that not only can each symptom be mapped to specific brain regions, but also the monoaminergic neurotransmitters that innervate the region (Figure 1). Moreover, Nemeroff [4] asserted that depression can be characterized specifically as a dysfunction of the limbic system-cerebrocortical circuits, all of which are modulated by the catecholaminergic monoamines.

depression-anxiety-Linking-Behavior-Neurobiology

Figure 1: Linking Behavior to Neurobiology.

Monoamine Hypothesis of Depression

The monoamine hypothesis of depression postulates that depression is caused by decreased function of 5-HT, NE, or DA in the brain [5]. Early evidence for the role of monoamines in depression came from clinical observations that treatment with reserpine, a drug that depletes monoamine neurotransmitters, evoked clinical symptoms very similar to major depression in patients who were not formerly depressed [6,7]. Further evidence came from studies in which MDD patients in remission previously relapsed and became depressed following monoamine depletion [8]. In addition, monoamine depletion resulted in worsened mood in individuals with a family history of MDD and in drug-free remitted individuals with MDD.

While the monoamine hypothesis of depression is a widely accepted hypothesis that has governed the development and selection of pharmacological agents used to treat depression, it does not explain the apparent lack of efficacy in a relatively large subset of patient who continue to suffer with specific residual symptoms including lack of interest or pleasure and who often exhibit lifespan comborbid psychological disorders.

Comorbid Conditions Commonly Associated with Depression

Most individuals with depression experience psychiatric comorbidity, according to the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCSR) [9]. Overall, 59.2% of respondents had comorbid anxiety disorders, 24.0% had comorbid substance use disorders, and 30.0% had comorbid impulse control disorders [10]. Among respondents with ADHD, there was significant comorbidity with a wide range of DSMIV disorders, with odds ratios of 2.7–7.5 for mood disorders, 1.5–5.5 for anxiety disorders, 1.5–7.9 for substance use disorders, and 3.7 for intermittent explosive disorder [9].

It is believed that the high comorbidity of MDD and substance use is due to common dysregulation in frontal-limbic systems associated with stress and reward pathways [11]. Evidence from neuroendocrine and neuroimaging studies indicate similar alterations in MDD and substance use, including decreased frontal metabolism [12-14], reduction in DA D2 receptors in frontal-striatal regions [14,15], reduced cell density and gray matter volume [13,16], hyperactivity in the amygdala [17-19], and hypoactivity in the anterior cingulate cortex [18,20,21] compared to healthy subjects.

Among the most studied of the illicit drugs that are abused is cocaine. Cocaine, like other stimulant substances, results in increased levels of DA within the nucleus accumbens, resulting in decreases in the brain stimulation reward threshold during use [22], and increased reward threshold during cocaine withdrawal [23]. During periods of abstinence, it has been reported that it is common for individuals to experience depression, fatigue (physical and mental), impaired concentration, and anxiety, often in association with withdrawal symptoms [24]. Dackis [25] asserted that cocaine withdrawal severity may result from cocaine-induced neurobiological changes and may identify patients with chronic hedonic dysregulation. Taken together this may explain the reaction of a subset of patients that, when questioned, note that cocaine increases focus, attention, and overall mood, thus suggesting these patients may be self-medicating with cocaine [26,27], and suggesting they are using cocaine (and other substances) in an attempt to manage their dysregulation. Often drug abusers will report the most positive effects on the first dose, no doubt as a result of pleasurable increases in phasic DA neuronal firing [28]. However, eventually the mesolimbic system will counteract this effect, with a compensatory tonic lowering of DA neuronal firing, resulting in withdrawal symptoms of feelings of intense sleepiness, anhedonia, and cravings for the agent between the doses [29]. Ultimately, the alterations in DA neuronal firing, results in a state of long lasting DA depletion, anxonal degeneration, which is directly associated with anhedonia and treatment-resistant depression [3].

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)

When MDD is comorbid with ADHD, it is associated with a much more severe course, an earlier age of illness onset, more complex psychiatric comorbidities, decreased quality of life, greater burden of illness, greater illness complexity, lower social functioning, lower work productivity, and lower employment rates than patients with MDD alone [30,31]. In addition, individuals with MDD and ADHD experience higher levels of substance use and psychosocial impairment than disorder in isolation [32-35], suggesting a potential variant of MDD, and therefore explaining the treatment resistance in this population when treated with traditional SSRIs.

Neuroimaging studies have identified morphological and functional features common to both MDD and ADHD, including decreased brain volumes and altered activity of frontal lobe structures (and specifically the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex). Interestingly, these areas have been associated with attention regulation, behavior selection, and emotion [36,37]. Neurotransmitter studies have revealed abnormalities in DA and NE signaling in both MDD and ADHD, suggesting a potentially shared underlying pathophysiology, at least in some individuals [38-41].

Support for the dopaminergic theory of ADHD which states that ADHD results from DA deficits and therefore lowered activity in the frontal cortex and striatum [42], comes from a variety of pharmacological and imaging studies which have revealed hypoperfusion and lowered activations in those areas [43-46]. Interestingly, with treatment with methlphenidate, normalization of the hypoperfusion of prefrontal areas is associated with the corresponding improvement in ADHD symptoms [47-51].

These findings point to a key role for DA dysfunction in the pathophysiology of ADHD and its comorbidities. With evidence pointing to dysfunctional neuromodulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in relation to executive dysfunction in ADHD [52-56], it is not too much of a stretch to consider low hedonic tone as an affective correlate in ADHD explaining the lack of responsiveness to SSRIs in depression.

Does the Presence of a Comorbid Substance Abuse Condition Predict Treatment Outcome?

Several studies published in the literature report worse treatment outcomes for depressed patients with comorbid substance abuse. Worthington et al. [57] observed that the degree of alcohol consumption at baseline in non-substance-abusing outpatients with MDD was a significant predictor of poorer response to treatment with the SSRI fluoxetine. In a similar study, chronic depression at baseline and no more than moderate alcohol consumption during therapy were predictors of poor response to fluoxetine among adolescents with comorbid depression and substance use disorder [58]. Further to this point, compared to elderly patients with depression alone, elderly patients with lifetime alcohol use and current depression have a lower rate of recovery from depression 1 month after hospital discharge, more hospital readmissions for depression, and are more likely to experience lack of improvement in depression at follow-up [59]. The impact of alcohol use on the long-term outcomes of MDD were reported by Mueller et al. [60] who observed that lifetime alcohol problems decreased the likelihood of recovery from depression from 2.3% to 1.2% over 10 years.

Similar findings have been observed for the impact of drug abuse on outcomes among depressed patients. The Collaborative Care for Anxiety and Panic (CCAP) study demonstrated that combined Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy (CBT) and pharmacotherapy resulted in improvement in depression regardless of cannabis use status, while usual care resulted in improvement in depression only for those using cannabis less than monthly [61].

Further support of this hypothesis was demonstrated in the STAR*D study, where the adult patients with MDD treated with the SSRI, citalopram, who used both alcohol and drugs had significantly reduced rates of remission and significantly increased times to reach remission compared to patients with MDD alone [62]. In the Treatment of Resistant Depression in Adolescents (TORDIA) trial, (which was designed to identify predictors of relapse and remission in adolescents with SSRI-resistant depression), higher drug and alcohol use at 12 weeks of treatment predicted non-remission (OR=2.07, p=0.007) at the end of the 24-week study period [63]. Not all studies, however, have supported the concept of far worse treatment outcomes for patients with comorbid substance use The Combining Medications to Enhance Depression Outcomes (CO-MED) study reported that patients with MDD and comorbid substance use disorder were as likely to respond and remit to treatment as those with MDD alone [64], perhaps suggesting that comorbid substance abuse may be related, but not causative of treatment resistance.

Interestingly, Tamm et al. [65] reported that a childhood diagnosis of ADHD, but not cannabis use in adulthood, was associated with executive dysfunction in young adults. Thus it was suggested that earlier initiation of cannabis use (before age 16) may be linked to poor cognitive outcomes and the presence of ADHD [65].

In examining this data, it might be hypothesized that comorbid substance abuse indicative of a comorbid ADHD, and may be predictive of treatment resistant depression.

Does the Presence of Comorbid ADHD Predict Treatment Outcome?

Only a few studies have investigated the moderating effects of comorbid ADHD on treatment outcomes for depression. Cunha et al. [53] reported that Cocaine Dependent Individuals (CDI) with ADHD presented with more pronounced executive alterations than and poorer cognitive functioning than controls, suggesting that pre-existing ADHD symptoms may have a significant negative impact on executive dysfunction in CDI.

Rohde et al. [66] assessed the negative impact of lifetime psychiatric comorbidity on participation in, and benefit from, CBT in adolescents with depression. Study investigators observed that patients with ADHD were more likely to relapse within the 24-month follow-up period than depressed patients without ADHD.

Birmaher et al. [67] observed response rates to paroxetine, imipramine and placebo over 8 weeks in depressed adolescents with and without ADHD. Patients with comorbid ADHD experienced significantly lower response rates, regardless of the treatment regimen, than patients with MDD alone (25% vs. 71%, respectively). Further to the point, the Treatment of Adolescent Depression Study (TADS) reported that comorbid disorders were a significant moderator of lowered response to treatment with fluoxetine, CBT, or combined fluoxetine and CBT in patients with MDD [68]. This latter study, however, did not specifically report on the moderating effects of co-morbid ADHD. It measured the moderating effect of a group of comorbid disorders that included ADHD as well as dysthymia and anxiety disorder. Taken together, the results of these studies suggest that ADHD may represent an important moderating influence on treatment outcomes for patients with MDD and comorbid ADHD. Furthermore, future studies may reveal if symptoms such as impulsivity or a pre-existing executive dysfunction could represent underlying cognitive endophenotypes that would substantially increase the risk for acquiring addictive disorders [53].

Shared Features of MDD, ADHD and Cocaine Abuse

Given the high rates of comorbidity between MDD, ADHD and the suggestions that increased impairment and worse outcomes are associated with their concurrence, a great deal of research has attempted to identify the mechanisms underlying this relationship. Recent evidence from genetic, neurological, and behavioral studies has revealed altered DA-regulated reward system functioning in both MDD and ADHD [69-72]. Because DA regulates mood [8], motivation [73], attention [74], and decision making [75], alterations in DA may lead to symptoms such as negative affect, low motivation, inattention, depression, and altered hedonic responsiveness [76-80] that are common to MDD and ADHD. Recent support for this hypothesis has come from work by Silvetti et al. [81], who used the Reward Value and Prediction Model (RVPM) to examine the profile of reinforcementrelated deficits in ADHD, suggesting that anterior cingulate cortex dysregulation might play a role in the pathogenesis of motivational deficits in ADHD.

Ultimately, while the literature has traditionally suggested that the comorbidity somehow blocks the improvement of depression from happening, one might also hypothesize that the comorbidity represents a different phenotypic depression entity that will not respond as well to typical SSRIs. Understanding that phenotypic presentation would require a specific review of the features perhaps associated with the common manifestations of this presentation.

Anhedonia

Anhedonia is generally defined as the inability to experience pleasure [82]. It is a central feature of MDD [83], but has also been studied in a variety of other illnesses including schizophrenia [84,85], substance use disorder [86,87], Parkinson’s disease [88,89], overeating [90,91], and risk-taking behaviors [92].

The term anhedonia, however, does not sufficiently capture the complex and multifaceted reward-associated deficits seen in neuropsychiatric disorders [93], but may manifest as a loss of interest or pleasure, and may preclude an individual from engaging in goal-oriented action. Investigators have identified poor hedonic responsiveness, manifested as altered sensitivity to reinforcement, in patients with ADHD [94], and suggest it as a common endophenotype for depression and the inattentive symptoms of ADHD [95].

Research into the neurobiology of anhedonia has focused on the centers in the brain related to reward and motivation, and have identified the dopaminergic and associated circuits as playing a key role [83,96,97]. Neurobiological studies have shown that lesions in reward and motivation circuits result in anhedonia [83,98-100] with neuroimaging studies demonstrating decreased activation in the medial frontal cortex as a possible causative factor underlying abnormal positive affect processing in patients [101-103].

Hedonic Tone

Changes in hedonic state (ie towards or away from happiness) can be understood as positive or negative relative to their most current hedonic state, with the magnitude of the shift determining the intensity of feelings. Happiness involves a pleasant level of hedonic tone, by virtue of a shift towards the positive pole, whereas, sadness represents movement toward the opposite or negative pole. This shared hedonic dimension allows many different feelings to interact or modulate each other. Interestingly, these positive and negative shifts in hedonic tone also define reward and punishment in terms of learning. Once an association is established, it will result in the hedonic shift or outcome expected with each expression of this behavior in the future. This link between feelings and learning appear to be established by output from the limbic system, specifically the pleasure (reward) pathway [104].

One can imagine that individuals differ in their baseline range of hedonic tone, also referred to as hedonic capacity or hedonic responsiveness defined as each individual’s ability to experience pleasure [105]. Interestingly, low hedonic tone is associated with several psychopathologies in addition to MDD, including schizophrenia, and substance use [105,106]. Variations in hedonic tone are also observed in healthy subjects [107].

Perhaps then one can imagine that those born with a low hedonic capacity would be more likely to require higher volumes of positive environmental activity to raise and maintain their lower natural baseline hedonic tone to a more euthymic tone. Thus, these individuals would either endeavor to increase their lower hedonic tone through highly stimulating (likely risky behaviors) or through the use of substances such as cocaine, that heighten hedonic tone or alternatively be forced to suffer with a chronic low mood. This is seen in the poor hedonic responsiveness common to MDD and ADHD [79,94,108,109]. The hypothesis of a low hedonic tone neurobiology is centered on the belief that there is a dysfunction in the mesolimbic DA system, which of course is known to act as the central reward system [70,110]. Further support for this view has come from a variety studies that have established poor hedonic tone as a feature of both depression and ADHD [79,94,108,109,111], as well as other catecholamine dysfunctional conditions and associated prefrontal hypoactivity including schizophrenia, anxiety disorders, suicidal ideation, successful suicide, chronic fatigue syndrome, and alcohol and substance abuse [112-119]. Anhedonia is also present in Parkinson’s disease, as well as in association with dysfunctional behaviors, such as overeating, and risktaking behavior [88,90,92].

Hypothesis: Anhedonia as State Versus Low Hedonic Tone as Trait

Anhedonia is a state of reduced ability to feel pleasure. It is the cornerstone symptom of depression and acts to direct research into possible treatment in treatment-resistant depression and/or double depression. Low hedonic tone, on the other hand, is the trait or genetic predisposition underlying one’s baseline range and lifelong characteristic ability to feel pleasure. Low hedonic tone would therefore be suggestive of a reduced lifetime capacity to experience euthymia and a lower likelihood of experiencing pleasure at any given time. Thus, it can be hypothesized that for those suffering with genetically lower set point or lower hedonic tone, there is an increasing likelihood of experiencing anhedonia, depression and/or dysthymia [120,121].

Thus, it follows that in response to this trait, individuals will need to do more to feel neutral or euthymic, resulting in an increased need for stimulation. In part, this may be seen as seeking external stimulation, such as through danger or risky behavior, or through internalizing (such as through fantasy), or through substance abuse (eg, cocaine), all designed to raise their current hedonic tone. Regardless of the choice to externally or internally raise their hedonic tone, patients with this trait would consistently attempt to cope by finding ways to maximize pleasure and raise mood from their low baseline tone. Furthermore, when stimulation is absent, they would experience a shift towards the more natural lower hedonic tone, and therefore a drop in their mood [122-124].

Thus much like lack of dopaminergic modulation in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex might contribute to executive dysfunction associated with ADHD, it follows that the same deficits in dopaminergic modulation in related areas might contribute to lowered hedonic tone, increased risk of depression, treatment-resistant depression and a higher risk of substance and specifically cocaine abuse.

Clinical Implications

While the hypothesis of low hedonic tone predicting a risk of developing anhedonia, depression, or dysthymia does not provide an etiological explanation for the development of these disorders, it may provide explanation of symptom overlap between and across ADHD and cocaine use within the MDD population. Additionally, low hedonic tone may act as a predictor of the presence of ADHD within a subset of patients presenting with symptoms of MDD, dysthymia, or double depression that have been deemed resistant to traditional medication and/or CBT [95,125,126].

As well, low hedonic tone may prove to be a predictor of future vulnerabilities and therefore may be an effective target for screening that can be used in the identification of a specific subtype of depressed individuals who are likely to be treatment resistant to typical management and who may require alternative treatment choices including psychostimulants [83,95].

As such, future studies will need to be conducted in order to confirm this hypothesis in adults. In addition, monitoring children for changes in mood and/or baseline hedonic experience in relation to activity level may help illuminate specific features that can help identify and therefore build resilience or adaptive behavior before the depressive psychopathology begins. In fact, it may be possible that correct use of psychostimulants in children may not specifically treat depression on their own, but may play a role in preventing the development of depression or drug abuse comorbidities (specifically stimulants, such as cocaine and amphetamines) [127,128] as well as act as an effective adjunct to traditional psychopharmacology and/or CBT in existing patients diagnosed with depression.

Conclusion

As evidence emerges that ADHD in childhood and adolescence is associated with increased risk of developing MDD in adulthood [129,130], there is an urgent need to identify the pathophysiologies common to these two disorders in an attempt to understand and successfully treat them both. Converging lines of evidence identify altered dopaminergic circuits as playing a key role in the pathophysiology of these disorders, leading to low hedonic tone and anhedonia. Thus, by implementing treatment strategies aimed at correcting low hedonic tone, clinicians can achieve not only remission from ADHD but also prevent these patients from progressing to MDD later in life. For patients with MDD, correcting low hedonic tone can be seen as one strategic decision to alleviating anhedonia and may serve to treat comorbid psychopathologies.

References

  1. Arguello PA, Gogos JA (2012) Genetic and cognitive windows into circuit mechanisms of psychiatric disease. Trends Neurosci 35: 3-13.
  2. Szabo ST, Gould TD, Manji HK (2013) The American Psychiatric Publishing Textbook of Psychopharmacolgy: Neurotransmitters, receptors, signal transduction, and second messengers. American Psychiatric Publishing Inc., Washington, DC.
  3. Stahl SM (2008) Stahl's essential psychopharmacology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  4. Nemeroff CB (2008) Recent findings in the pathophysiology of depression. FOCUS: J Lifelong Learn Psychiatry 6: 3-14.
  5. Krishnan V, Nestler EJ (2008) The molecular neurobiology of depression. Nature 455: 894-902.
  6. Patten SB, Barbui C (2004) Drug-induced depression: a systematic review to inform clinical practice. Psychother Psychosom 73: 207-215.
  7. Peterfy G, Pinter EJ, Pattee CJ (1976) Psychosomatic aspects of catecholamine depletion: comparative studies of metabolic, endocrine and affective changes. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1: 243-253.
  8. Ruhé HG, Mason NS, Schene AH (2007) Mood is indirectly related to serotonin, norepinephrine and dopamine levels in humans: a meta-analysis of monoamine depletion studies. Mol Psychiatry 12: 331-359.
  9. Kessler RC, Adler L, Barkley R, Biederman J, Conners CK, et al. (2006) The prevalence and correlates of adult ADHD in the United States: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Am J Psychiatry 163: 716-723.
  10. Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, Jin R, Koretz D, et al. (2003) The epidemiology of major depressive disorder: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R). JAMA 289: 3095-3105.
  11. Brady KT, Sinha R (2005) Co-occurring mental and substance use disorders: the neurobiological effects of chronic stress. Am J Psychiatry 162: 1483-1493.
  12. Brody AL, Barsom MW, Bota RG, Saxena S (2001) Prefrontal-subcortical and limbic circuit mediation of major depressive disorder. Semin Clin Neuropsychiatry 6: 102-112.
  13. Franklin TR, Acton PD, Maldjian JA, Gray JD, Croft JR, et al. (2002) Decreased gray matter concentration in the insular, orbitofrontal, cingulate, and temporal cortices of cocaine patients. Biol Psychiatry 51: 134-142.
  14. Volkow ND, Fowler JS (2000) Addiction, a disease of compulsion and drive: involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 10: 318-325.
  15. Laasonen-Balk T, Kuikka J, Viinamäki H, Husso-Saastamoinen M, Lehtonen J, et al. (1999) Striatal dopamine transporter density in major depression. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 144: 282-285.
  16. Cotter D, Mackay D, Landau S, Kerwin R, Everall I (2001) Reduced glial cell density and neuronal size in the anterior cingulate cortex in major depressive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 58: 545-553.
  17. Childress AR, Mozley PD, McElgin W, Fitzgerald J, Reivich M, et al. (1999) Limbic activation during cue-induced cocaine craving. Am J Psychiatry 156: 11-18.
  18. Davidson RJ, Lewis DA, Alloy LB, Amaral DG, Bush G, et al. (2002) Neural and behavioral substrates of mood and mood regulation. Biol Psychiatry 52: 478-502.
  19. Kilts CD, Schweitzer JB, Quinn CK, Gross RE, Faber TL, et al. (2001) Neural activity related to drug craving in cocaine addiction. Arch Gen Psychiatry 58: 334-341.
  20. Kaufman JN, Ross TJ, Stein EA, Garavan H (2003) Cingulate hypoactivity in cocaine users during a GO-NOGO task as revealed by event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci 23: 7839-7843.
  21. Sinha R, Lacadie C, Skudlarski P, Fulbright RK, Rounsaville BJ, et al. (2005) Neural activity associated with stress-induced cocaine craving: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 183: 171-180.
  22. Kornetsky C, Porrino LJ (1992) Brain mechanisms of drug-induced reinforcement. Res Publ Assoc Res Nerv Ment Dis 70: 59-77.
  23. Koob GF, Ahmed SH, Boutrel B, Chen SA, Kenny PJ, et al. (2004) Neurobiological mechanisms in the transition from drug use to drug dependence. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 27: 739-749.
  24. Coffey SF, Dansky BS, Carrigan MH, Brady KT (2000) Acute and protracted cocaine abstinence in an outpatient population: a prospective study of mood, sleep and withdrawal symptoms. Drug Alcohol Depend 59: 277-286.
  25. Dackis CA (2005) New treatments for cocaine abuse. Drug Discov Today Ther Strateg 2: 79-86.
  26. Müller CP, Schumann G (2011) Drugs as instruments: a new framework for non-addictive psychoactive drug use. Behav Brain Sci 34: 293-310.
  27. Müller CP, Schumann G (2011) To use or not to use: Expanding the view on non-addictive psychoactive drug consumption and its implications. Behav Brain Sci 34: 328-347.
  28. Foltin RW, Haney M (2004) Intranasal cocaine in humans: acute tolerance, cardiovascular and subjective effects. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 78: 93-101.
  29. Gawin FH (1991) Cocaine addiction: psychology and neurophysiology. Science 251: 1580-1586.
  30. Bond DJ, Hadjipavlou G, Lam RW, McIntyre RS, Beaulieu S, et al. (2012) The Canadian Network for Mood and Anxiety Treatments (CANMAT) task force recommendations for the management of patients with mood disorders and comorbid attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Ann Clin Psychiatry 24: 23-37.
  31. McIntyre RS, Kennedy SH, Soczynska JK, Nguyen HT, Bilkey TS, et al. (2010) Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults with bipolar disorder or major depressive disorder: results from the international mood disorders collaborative project. Prim Care Companion J Clin Psychiatry 12.
  32. Biederman J, Mick E, Faraone SV (1998) Depression in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) children: "true" depression or demoralization? J Affect Disord 47: 113-122.
  33. Biederman J, Ball SW, Monuteaux MC, Mick E, Spencer TJ, et al. (2008) New insights into the comorbidity between ADHD and major depression in adolescent and young adult females. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 47: 426-434.
  34. Chronis-Tuscano A, Molina BS, Pelham WE, Applegate B, Dahlke A, et al. (2010) Very early predictors of adolescent depression and suicide attempts in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 67: 1044-1051.
  35. Warden D, Riggs PD, Min SJ, Mikulich-Gilbertson SK, Tamm L, et al. (2012) Major depression and treatment response in adolescents with ADHD and substance use disorder. Drug Alcohol Depend 120: 214-219.
  36. Konarski JZ, McIntyre RS, Kennedy SH, Rafi-Tari S, Soczynska JK, et al. (2008) Volumetric neuroimaging investigations in mood disorders: bipolar disorder versus major depressive disorder. Bipolar Disord 10: 1-37.
  37. Seidman LJ, Valera EM, Makris N, Monuteaux MC, Boriel DL, et al. (2006) Dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex volumetric abnormalities in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder identified by magnetic resonance imaging. Biol Psychiatry 60: 1071-1080.
  38. Dunlop BW, Nemeroff CB (2007) The role of dopamine in the pathophysiology of depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 64: 327-337.
  39. Garnock-Jones KP, Keating GM (2010) Spotlight on atomoxetine in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. CNS Drugs 24: 85-88.
  40. Nigg JT, Casey BJ (2005) An integrative theory of attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder based on the cognitive and affective neurosciences. Dev Psychopathol 17: 785-806.
  41. Sergeant JA, Geurts H, Huijbregts S, Scheres A, Oosterlaan J (2003) The top and the bottom of ADHD: a neuropsychological perspective. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 27: 583-592.
  42. Levy F (1991) The dopamine theory of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Aust N Z J Psychiatry 25: 277-283.
  43. Kim BN, Lee JS, Shin MS, Cho SC, Lee DS (2002) Regional cerebral perfusion abnormalities in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Statistical parametric mapping analysis. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 252: 219-225.
  44. Lou HC, Henriksen L, Bruhn P (1984) Focal cerebral hypoperfusion in children with dysphasia and/or attention deficit disorder. Arch Neurol 41: 825-829.
  45. Lou HC, Henriksen L, Bruhn P, Børner H, Nielsen JB (1989) Striatal dysfunction in attention deficit and hyperkinetic disorder. Arch Neurol 46: 48-52.
  46. Lou HC, Henriksen L, Bruhn P (1990) Focal cerebral dysfunction in developmental learning disabilities. Lancet 335: 8-11.
  47. Kim BN, Lee JS, Cho SC, Lee DS (2001) Methylphenidate increased regional cerebral blood flow in subjects with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Yonsei Med J 42: 19-29.
  48. Lee JS, Kim BN, Kang E, Lee DS, Kim YK, et al. (2005) Regional cerebral blood flow in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: comparison before and after methylphenidate treatment. Hum Brain Mapp 24: 157-164.
  49. Matochik JA, Nordahl TE, Gross M, Semple WE, King AC, et al. (1993) Effects of acute stimulant medication on cerebral metabolism in adults with hyperactivity. Neuropsychopharmacology 8: 377-386.
  50. Matochik JA, Liebenauer LL, King AC, Szymanski HV, Cohen RM, et al. (1994) Cerebral glucose metabolism in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder after chronic stimulant treatment. Am J Psychiatry 151: 658-664.
  51. Vaidya CJ, Austin G, Kirkorian G, Ridlehuber HW, Desmond JE, et al. (1998) Selective effects of methylphenidate in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a functional magnetic resonance study. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95: 14494-14499.
  52. Madhoo M, Keefe RS, Roth RM, Sambunaris A, Wu J, et al. (2013) Lisdexamfetamine Dimesylate Augmentation in Adults With Persistent Executive Dysfunction After Partial or Full Remission of Major Depressive Disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology.
  53. Cunha PJ, Gonçalves PD, Ometto M, Dos Santos B, Nicastri S, et al. (2013) Executive cognitive dysfunction and ADHD in cocaine dependence: searching for a common cognitive endophenotype for addictive disorders. Front Psychiatry 4: 126.
  54. Fuermaier AB, Tucha L, Koerts J, Aschenbrenner S, Westermann C, et al. (2013) Complex prospective memory in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. PLoS One 8: e58338.
  55. Missonnier P, Hasler R, Perroud N, Herrmann FR, Millet P, et al. (2013) EEG anomalies in adult ADHD subjects performing a working memory task. Neuroscience 241: 135-146.
  56. Qian Y, Shuai L, Chan RC, Qian QJ, Wang Y (2013) The developmental trajectories of executive function of children and adolescents with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Res Dev Disabil 34: 1434-1445.
  57. Worthington J, Fava M, Agustin C, Alpert J, Nierenberg AA, et al. (1996) Consumption of alcohol, nicotine, and caffeine among depressed outpatients. Relationship with response to treatment. Psychosomatics 37: 518-522.
  58. Hirschtritt ME, Pagano ME, Christian KM, McNamara NK, Stansbrey RJ, et al. (2012) Moderators of fluoxetine treatment response for children and adolescents with comorbid depression and substance use disorders. J Subst Abuse Treat 42: 366-372.
  59. Cook BL, Winokur G, Garvey MJ, Beach V (1991) Depression and previous alcoholism in the elderly. Br J Psychiatry 158: 72-75.
  60. Mueller TI, Lavori PW, Keller MB, Swartz A, Warshaw M, et al. (1994) Prognostic effect of the variable course of alcoholism on the 10-year course of depression. Am J Psychiatry 151: 701-706.
  61. Bricker JB, Russo J, Stein MB, Sherbourne C, Craske M, et al. (2007) Does occasional cannabis use impact anxiety and depression treatment outcomes?: Results from a randomized effectiveness trial. Depress Anxiety 24: 392-398.
  62. Davis LL, Wisniewski SR, Howland RH, Trivedi MH, Husain MM, et al. (2010) Does comorbid substance use disorder impair recovery from major depression with SSRI treatment? An analysis of the STAR*D level one treatment outcomes. Drug Alcohol Depend 107: 161-170.
  63. Emslie GJ, Mayes T, Porta G, Vitiello B, Clarke G, et al. (2010) Treatment of Resistant Depression in Adolescents (TORDIA): week 24 outcomes. Am J Psychiatry 167: 782-791.
  64. Davis LL, Pilkinton P, Wisniewski SR, Trivedi MH, Gaynes BN, et al. (2012) Effect of concurrent substance use disorder on the effectiveness of single and combination antidepressant medications for the treatment of major depression: an exploratory analysis of a single-blind randomized trial. Depress Anxiety 29: 111-122.
  65. Tamm L, Epstein JN, Lisdahl KM, Molina B, Tapert S, et al. (2013) Impact of ADHD and cannabis use on executive functioning in young adults. Drug Alcohol Depend 133: 607-614.
  66. Rohde P, Clarke GN, Lewinsohn PM, Seeley JR, Kaufman NK (2001) Impact of comorbidity on a cognitive-behavioral group treatment for adolescent depression. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 40: 795-802.
  67. Birmaher B, McCafferty JP, Bellew KM, Beebe KL, Co-morbid ADHD and disruptive behavior disorders as predictors of response in adolescents treated for major depression. Abstract presented at American Psychiatric Association Annual Meeting, Chicago, May.
  68. Curry J, Rohde P, Simons A, Silva S, Vitiello B, et al. (2006) Predictors and moderators of acute outcome in the Treatment for Adolescents with Depression Study (TADS). J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 45: 1427-1439.
  69. Genro JP, Kieling C, Rohde LA, Hutz MH (2010) Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and the dopaminergic hypotheses. Expert Rev Neurother 10: 587-601.
  70. Martin-Soelch C (2009) Is depression associated with dysfunction of the central reward system? Biochem Soc Trans 37: 313-317.
  71. Olino TM, McMakin DL, Dahl RE, Ryan ND, Silk JS, et al. (2011) "I won, but I'm not getting my hopes up": depression moderates the relationship of outcomes and reward anticipation. Psychiatry Res 194: 393-395.
  72. Opmeer EM, Kortekaas R, Aleman A (2010) Depression and the role of genes involved in dopamine metabolism and signalling. Prog Neurobiol 92: 112-133.
  73. Salamone JD, Correa M (2012) The mysterious motivational functions of mesolimbic dopamine. Neuron 76: 470-485.
  74. Noudoost B, Moore T (2011) The role of neuromodulators in selective attention. Trends Cogn Sci 15: 585-591.
  75. Assadi SM, Yücel M, Pantelis C (2009) Dopamine modulates neural networks involved in effort-based decision-making. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 33: 383-393.
  76. Beauchaine TP, Neuhaus E, Brenner SL, Gatzke-Kopp L (2008) Ten good reasons to consider biological processes in prevention and intervention research. Dev Psychopathol 20: 745-774.
  77. Durston S (2003) A review of the biological bases of ADHD: what have we learned from imaging studies? Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 9: 184-195.
  78. Epstein J, Pan H, Kocsis JH, Yang Y, Butler T, et al. (2006) Lack of ventral striatal response to positive stimuli in depressed versus normal subjects. Am J Psychiatry 163: 1784-1790.
  79. Pizzagalli DA, Iosifescu D, Hallett LA, Ratner KG, Fava M (2008) Reduced hedonic capacity in major depressive disorder: evidence from a probabilistic reward task. J Psychiatr Res 43: 76-87.
  80. Scheres A, Milham MP, Knutson B, Castellanos FX (2007) Ventral striatal hyporesponsiveness during reward anticipation in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 61: 720-724.
  81. Silvetti M, Wiersema JR, Sonuga-Barke E, Verguts T (2013) Deficient reinforcement learning in medial frontal cortex as a model of dopamine-related motivational deficits in ADHD. Neural Netw 46: 199-209.
  82. Gorwood P (2008) Neurobiological mechanisms of anhedonia. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 10: 291-299.
  83. Stein DJ (2008) Depression, anhedonia, and psychomotor symptoms: the role of dopaminergic neurocircuitry. CNS Spectr 13: 561-565.
  84. Ritsner MS, Arbitman M, Lisker A (2011) Anhedonia is an important factor of health-related quality-of-life deficit in schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis 199: 845-853.
  85. Wolf DH (2006) Anhedonia in schizophrenia. Curr Psychiatry Rep 8: 322-328.
  86. Koob GF, Le Moal M (2001) Drug addiction, dysregulation of reward, and allostasis. Neuropsychopharmacology 24: 97-129.
  87. Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ, Goldstein RZ (2002) Role of dopamine, the frontal cortex and memory circuits in drug addiction: insight from imaging studies. Neurobiol Learn Mem 78: 610-624.
  88. Assogna F, Cravello L, Caltagirone C, Spalletta G (2011) Anhedonia in Parkinson's disease: a systematic review of the literature. Mov Disord 26: 1825-1834.
  89. Loas G, Krystkowiak P, Godefroy O (2012) Anhedonia in Parkinson's disease: an overview. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 24: 444-451.
  90. Davis C, Woodside DB (2002) Sensitivity to the rewarding effects of food and exercise in the eating disorders. Compr Psychiatry 43: 189-194.
  91. Davis C, Strachan S, Berkson M (2004) Sensitivity to reward: implications for overeating and overweight. Appetite 42: 131-138.
  92. Franken IH, Zijlstra C, Muris P (2006) Are nonpharmacological induced rewards related to anhedonia? A study among skydivers. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 30: 297-300.
  93. Der-Avakian A, Markou A (2012) The neurobiology of anhedonia and other reward-related deficits. Trends Neurosci 35: 68-77.
  94. Luman M, Oosterlaan J, Sergeant JA (2005) The impact of reinforcement contingencies on AD/HD: a review and theoretical appraisal. Clin Psychol Rev 25: 183-213.
  95. Meinzer MC, Pettit JW, Leventhal AM, Hill RM (2012) Explaining the covariance between attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms and depressive symptoms: the role of hedonic responsivity. J Clin Psychol 68: 1111-1121.
  96. Bevins RA, Besheer J (2005) Novelty reward as a measure of anhedonia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29: 707-714.
  97. Knutson B, Fong GW, Adams CM, Varner JL, Hommer D (2001) Dissociation of reward anticipation and outcome with event-related fMRI. Neuroreport 12: 3683-3687.
  98. Bhatia KP, Marsden CD (1994) The behavioural and motor consequences of focal lesions of the basal ganglia in man. Brain 117: 859-876.
  99. Miller JM, Vorel SR, Tranguch AJ, Kenny ET, Mazzoni P, et al. (2006) Anhedonia after a selective bilateral lesion of the globus pallidus. Am J Psychiatry 163: 786-788.
  100. Vijayaraghavan L, Vaidya JG, Humphreys CT, Beglinger LJ, Paradiso S (2008) Emotional and motivational changes after bilateral lesions of the globus pallidus. Neuropsychology 22: 412-418.
  101. Harvey PO, Pruessner J, Czechowska Y, Lepage M (2007) Individual differences in trait anhedonia: a structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging study in non-clinical subjects. Mol Psychiatry 12: 703, 767-775.
  102. Keedwell PA, Andrew C, Williams SC, Brammer MJ, Phillips ML (2005) The neural correlates of anhedonia in major depressive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 58: 843-853.
  103. Mitterschiffthaler MT, Kumari V, Malhi GS, Brown RG, Giampietro VP, et al. (2003) Neural response to pleasant stimuli in anhedonia: an fMRI study. Neuroreport 14: 177-182.
  104. LeDoux JE (2000) Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci 23: 155-184.
  105. Snaith RP, Hamilton M, Morley S, Humayan A, Hargreaves D, et al. (1995) A scale for the assessment of hedonic tone the Snaith-Hamilton Pleasure Scale. Br J Psychiatry 167: 99-103.
  106. Snaith RP (1993) Identifying depression: the significance of anhedonia. Hosp Pract (Off Ed) 28: 55-60.
  107. Jankowski KS, Ciarkowska W (2008) Diurnal variation in energetic arousal, tense arousal, and hedonic tone in extreme morning and evening types. Chronobiol Int 25: 577-595.
  108. Forbes EE, Dahl RE (2005) Neural systems of positive affect: relevance to understanding child and adolescent depression? Dev Psychopathol 17: 827-850.
  109. Pizzagalli DA, Jahn AL, O'Shea JP (2005) Toward an objective characterization of an anhedonic phenotype: a signal-detection approach. Biol Psychiatry 57: 319-327.
  110. Bressan RA, Crippa JA (2005) The role of dopamine in reward and pleasure behaviour--review of data from preclinical research. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl : 14-21.
  111. Bogdan R, Pizzagalli DA (2006) Acute stress reduces reward responsiveness: implications for depression. Biol Psychiatry 60: 1147-1154.
  112. Fawcett J (1993) The morbidity and mortality of clinical depression. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 8: 217-220.
  113. Gardner EL (2011) Addiction and brain reward and antireward pathways. Adv Psychosom Med 30: 22-60.
  114. Janiri L, Martinotti G, Dario T, Reina D, Paparello F, et al. (2005) Anhedonia and substance-related symptoms in detoxified substance-dependent subjects: a correlation study. Neuropsychobiology 52: 37-44.
  115. Martinotti G, Carli V, Tedeschi D, Di Giannantonio M, Roy A, et al. (2009) Mono- and polysubstance dependent subjects differ on social factors, childhood trauma, personality, suicidal behaviour, and comorbid Axis I diagnoses. Addict Behav 34: 790-793.
  116. Mason O, Startup M, Halpin S, Schall U, Conrad A, et al. (2004) Risk factors for transition to first episode psychosis among individuals with 'at-risk mental states'. Schizophr Res 71: 227-237.
  117. Spijker J, de Graaf R, Ten Have M, Nolen WA, Speckens A (2010) Predictors of suicidality in depressive spectrum disorders in the general population: results of the Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 45: 513-521.
  118. Thomas M, Smith A (2009) An investigation into the cognitive deficits associated with chronic fatigue syndrome. Open Neurol J 3: 13-23.
  119. Watson D, Naragon-Gainey K (2010) On the specificity of positive emotional dysfunction in psychopathology: evidence from the mood and anxiety disorders and schizophrenia/schizotypy. Clin Psychol Rev 30: 839-848.
  120. Di Nicola M, De Risio L, Battaglia C, Camardese G, Tedeschi D, et al. (2013) Reduced hedonic capacity in euthymic bipolar subjects: a trait-like feature? J Affect Disord 147: 446-450.
  121. Loas G (1996) Vulnerability to depression: a model centered on anhedonia. J Affect Disord 41: 39-53.
  122. Barrett LF (1996) Hedonic tone, perceived arousal, and item desirability: Three components of self-reported mood. Cogn Emot 10: 47-68.
  123. Feldman LA (1995) Variations in the circumplex structure of mood. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 21: 806-817.
  124. Newell SE, Harries P, Ayers S (2012) Boredom proneness in a psychiatric inpatient population. Int J Soc Psychiatry 58: 488-495.
  125. McMakin DL, Olino TM, Porta G, Dietz LJ, Emslie G, et al. (2012) Anhedonia predicts poorer recovery among youth with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor treatment-resistant depression. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 51: 404-411.
  126. Rubin DH (2012) Joy returns last: anhedonia and treatment resistance in depressed adolescents. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 51: 353-355.
  127. Beardslee WR, Chien PL, Bell CC (2011) Prevention of mental disorders, substance abuse, and problem behaviors: a developmental perspective. Psychiatr Serv 62: 247-254.
  128. Southwick SM, Vythilingam M, Charney DS (2005) The psychobiology of depression and resilience to stress: implications for prevention and treatment. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 1: 255-291.
  129. Diler RS, Daviss WB, Lopez A, Axelson D, Iyengar S, et al. (2007) Differentiating major depressive disorder in youths with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Affect Disord 102: 125-130.
  130. Meinzer MC, Lewinsohn PM, Pettit JW, Seeley JR, Gau JM, et al. (2013) Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adolescence predicts onset of major depressive disorder through early adulthood. Depress Anxiety 30: 546-553.
Citation: Sternat T, Lodzinski A, Katzman MA (2014) Hedonic Tone: A Bridge between the Psychobiology of Depression and its Comorbidities. J Depress Anxiety 3:147.

Copyright: © 2014 Sternat T, et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Top