ISSN: 2165-7556
+44 1300 500008
Research Article - (2013) Volume 3, Issue 1
Keywords: Driving simulator; Lateral dynamics; Visuo-vestibular interaction; Virtual reality; Sensory cues conflict
Maintaining a sustainable reality of the represented dynamics is a very difficult and sometimes even an impossible issue by using driving simulators. The major leading reasons of this problem are the constrained workspace of the driving simulator and whether a motion platform exists integrated with the driving simulator. The first driving simulators were fixed-base and the simulation was principally only realized by the visual stimulus [1,2] to constitute the self-motion perception. This perception is based on the displacement of visual scene flow on the retina referring to the information about the velocity, direction of the motion and the relative distances [3].
For the static simulator case, illusory self-motion ‘vection’ often occurs because the driver is stationary and the visual scenario is moving [4-12].
It is obvious that inertial restitution addresses a significant role to maintain a developed fidelity of the driver behaviours on driving simulators. The dynamic simulators are being used since the mid 1960’s (Stewart platform) [13] firstly for the flight simulators, then the use has spread to the automotive applications [14-20]. The utilization scope diversifies from driver training to research purposes such as vehicle dynamics control, advanced driving assistance systems (ADAS) [21].
Subjects prefer verbally the dynamic platform rather than the static case [22-25]. Driving simulation sickness was assessed between dynamic and static simulators in some studies [22,26,27]. It was declared that, simulation sickness was coincided lower when using dynamic simulators rather than static simulators respectively [8,26,27]. Siegler et al. in 2001 stated that if the motion platform was activated, the bias in reaching increased levels of decelerations was reduced strongly comparing to inactivated platform case for a braking maneuver [28] which was an indicator for avoidance of visuo-vestibular cues conflict. Berger et al. investigated the believability of the forward acceleration on a Stewart motion platform [18,19].
However, there is a lack of publications on reality of lateral dynamics in terms of multi sensory levels (vehicle model: visual lateral acceleration ayv=ay_veh, motion platform: inertial lateral acceleration, human head level: vestibular lateral acceleration ay_vest=ay, sensed cues). Because of that fact, the reality of lateral dynamics in absence and in presence of the motion platform that yield to two conditions was surveyed in this article.
This paper surveys if there is any correlation between the visuovestibular level accelerations in case of static and dynamic simulators. Visuo-vestibular level accelerations stand for the real-time registered and measured visual and vestibular level accelerations.
Thus it aims to prove the reduction of simulator sickness by using motion platform in terms of visuo-vestibular level acceleration cues proximity (positive correlation).
Here, the visual level acceleration refers to the lateral acceleration values registered from the vehicle model that moves in the visual environment for the driving simulator. Whereas the vestibular level acceleration represents the subjects’ head lateral accelerations connected to the right ear by using a head phone.
Also, perception fidelity was investigated by correlating the vestibular level sensed lateral accelerations with the subjective impression which was measured by using the proposed questionnaire.
The research method tackled in this study was to compare the motion platform’s contribution on visuo-vestibular acceleration conflicts.
Furthermore the subjects were asked to neither steer the steering wheel nor use the gas, brake pedals and the gear.
The throughout experiment phases were realized as in “virtual driver” mode in the driving simulation software SCANeR studio (Figure 1).
In the data analysis part, the superposition principle of motion was used for evaluating the sensed lateral dynamics at head (vestibular) level (Figures 2-5).
The measured lateral acceleration in vestibular level is calculated by equation (1) where
ay,sensed : Sensed lateral acceleration (m/s2)
ayv : Lateral translational acceleration at vestibular level (m/s2)
φ : Roll angle at vestibular level (°)
g : Gravitational acceleration (m/s2)
……. (1)
ay,sensed was measured from the participants’ right ear levels for the same driven scenario for the static and dynamic platforms via using the sensor in figure 5.
Dynamic driving simulator
This research work was performed under the dynamic as well as static operations of the SAAM (Simulateur Automobile Arts et Métiers) driving simulator (Figure 2). The dynamic driving simulator SAAM involves a 6 DOF (degree of freedom) motion system (Figure 3 and table 1) [15]. It is operated on a RENAULT Twingo 2 cabin with the original control instruments (gas, brake pedals, steering wheel). The visual system is realized by a 150° cylindrical view (Figure 2). With the driving cabin of the simulator, the multi-level measuring techniques are available: vehicle model and motion platform dynamics levels real-time data acquisition via SCANeR studio driving simulation software, vestibular level dynamics real-time data acquisition via XSens motion tracker, arm and neck muscles dynamics measurement via Biopac EMG (electromyography) device, human’s center of gravity displacements measuring equipment Technoconcept to check postural stability [1,29,30].
DOF | Displacement | Velocity | Acceleration |
---|---|---|---|
Pitch | ± 22 deg | ± 30 deg/s | ± 500 deg/s2 |
Roll | ± 21 deg | ± 30 deg/s | ± 500 deg/s2 |
Yaw | ± 22 deg | ± 40 deg/s | ± 400 deg/s2 |
Heave | ± 0.18 m | ± 0.30 m/s | ± 0.5 g |
Surge | ± 0.25 m | ± 0.5 m/s | ± 0.6 g |
Sway | ± 0.25 m | ± 0.5 m/s | ± 0.6 g |
Table 1: Limits of each degree of freedom (DOF) of the dynamic driving simulator SAAM.
Figure 2 illustrates the SAAM moving-base driving simulator. It could be operated as with static or dynamic platform by switching the “motion platform” module off and on respectively. As seen in the figure, in general there are three dynamical systems of the SAAM driving simulator. They are vehicle dynamics, motion platform dynamics (motion system) and human vestibular dynamics (proprioceptive system). By manipulating or controlling the vehicle dynamics that moves in the vision system and the motion platform dynamics via motion drive algorithms, their effect on human vestibular dynamics can be compared.
In this article, the effect of having an inertial stimulus (motion platform is active and passive separately for the same driving scenario in figures 6 and 7) on human vestibular dynamics and the perception fidelity were discussed [14].
ay,sensed measured to obtain the vestibular level lateral accelerations of the subjects and ayv, which was given in figure 4, registered from the vehicle model driven in real-time via the driving simulation software for the same driven scenario for the static and dynamic platforms via using the sensor in figure 5.
Figure 5 describes the motion cueing algorithm used for the dynamic platform condition in this study. The motion cueing algorithm was included in the SCANeR studio driving simulation software via DLL plugin in order to accomplish the real-time driving experiments with the participations of the subjects.
Vestibular level data acquisition
In order to save the acceleration data from the vestibular level, a motion tracking sensor was used (Figure 5). This motion tracker can measure the data such as the roll, pitch, yaw angles and rates as well as the accelerations in X, Y and Z. The data are calibrated due to three dimensional quaternion orientation. The sampling rate for the data registration during the sensor measurements was 20 Hz. For the calibrated data acquisition, the alignment reset was chosen which simply combined the object and the heading reset at a single instant in time. This had the advantage that all co-ordinate systems could be aligned with a single action [16]. The details about the XSens motion tracking sensor are given in [16].
ay_vest and ay,sensed are equal to each other and they represent the measurements at the participants’ ears as in figure 6 relying on the superposition principle of the translational and the rotational motions based on equation 1.
Vehicle level data acquisition
Vehicle level data registered by SCANeR studio software can be splitted as; command data (steering wheel angle, gas, brake pedal input, etc.), motion platform level (translational and angular accelerations of the hexapod platform),vehicle level data (vehicle dynamics, engine, etc.), frequential analysis of the motion platform and vehicle levels (by using FFT (Fast Fourier Transform)).
Protocol
Two conditions were driven by using “virtual driver”. This was a kind of driving of which the driving simulator was driven with “autopilot”. In order to achieve this goal, we chose and used a driver handler option that was already appointed as “follow a speed target specified from command data”.
The experiment protocol involved two phases of the driving situations as static and dynamic platform conditions on a country road (Figure 6).
Figure 6 also depicts the X-Y trajectory which was attempted in the experiment phases. The whole experimental phase was completed with a constant velocity of 60 km/h in 126 seconds (Figure 7).
Subjects
The experimental procedure was done for static and dynamic platform cases. 37 subjects (N=37, 29 males and 8 females) participated in experiments.
For the static platform condition, 16 subjects (N=16, 11 male and 5 female participants) aged (mean: 33.44 years, SD: 7.66 years) and with driving licence experience (mean : 15.03 years, SD: 7.14 years) participated in this phase (SD: standard deviation).
For the dynamic platform condition, 21 subjects (N=21, 18 male and 3 female participants) aged (mean: 31.62 years, SD: 7.33 years) and with driving licence experience (mean: 13.07 years, SD: 6.90 years) participated in this phase (SD: standard deviation).
The effect of having an inertial stimulus (motion platform) on proximity to the reality was discussed here for the sensed lateral dynamics regarding driving simulators.
In order to assess this, the vestibular and vehicle level lateral accelerations (ay) were collected by using a motion tracking sensor (Figure 5) and SCANeR studio software respectively.
Pearson’s correlation was computed [18] between the conditions of static and dynamic driving simulator situations in order to assess the visuo-vestibular sensory conflict levels. According this; if the lateral acceleration at vehicle level is negatively correlated to the lateral acceleration at vestibular level, it yields less realistic driving simulation session. And if they are positively correlated with each other, it shows a convergence to the reality.
A matlab code example used for the Pearson’s correlation is given below:
* [rvisves, pvisves] =corrcoef (ay_vest_classic_filt, ay_veh_static)
Code (*) shows a matlab code to maintain the correlation coefficients between the vestibular and vehicle level lateral accelerations to establish (Table 3). This process was applied to every single subject and to the both cases.
ay_vest_classic_filt: vestibular level lateral acceleration which was registered at 20 Hz with classical motion cueing algorithm (dynamic platform)
ay_veh_static: vehicle level lateral acceleration which was registered at 20 Hz
rvisves: correlation coefficient between the vestibular and vehicle level lateral accelerations
pvisves: probability (p-value)
Multiple linear regressions were assigned to evaluate the sensory cue conflict in both situations (Figures 1 and 2) and their difference was compared with a two-tailed Mann-Whitney U test, which is a nonparametric hypothesis test, by using XLSTAT statistics software.
Finally, the subjective evaluations (questionnaire) and objective measurements from the head level (ay_vest=ay_sensed) were checked in terms of Pearson’s correlation coefficient in order to discuss the perception fidelity for the both cases.
In this section, the associations of lateral accelerations on the vehicle and the vestibular levels (Figure 8) and their level of significance were discussed as of having and not having the motion platform during the driving simulator operations.
Figure 8 explained briefly the impact of the inertial stimulus (motion platform) as an example for a subject. The blue curve illustrated the vestibularly sensed lateral acceleration from the experiments which could also be computed from equation 1. The red curve was depicting the lateral acceleration of the vehicle center of gravity and it was same for the both cases.
Depending on these graphs, it was seen that the lateral acceleration sensed by the vestibular organ indicated a high disparity from the vehicle lateral acceleration between the curvatures (45-105s), during the second curvature turn (105-110s) and the end sections (110-126s) (Figures 6-8) for the static platform condition whereas there was a close match of visuo-vestibular cues for the dynamic platform case.
Moreover between 65-85s. and on the second curvature turn, the high frequent motion was coincided in a higher severity at the vestibular level of the participants for the static platform compared to the dynamic condition (Figure 8).
The visuo-vestibular lateral accelerations’ gap reduction during the second curvature turn (105-110s) was sourcing from the onset cueing in general. The close visuo-vestibular lateral acceleration fit in the end sections (110-126s) (Figures 6-8) arose by the tilt coordination and the time delays which were integrated in real-time as seen in figure 4, tables 1 and 2 for the dynamic platform condition whereas there was a mismatch of visuo-vestibular cues for the static platform case.
Symbol | Longitudinal | Lateral | Roll | Pitch | Yaw |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
2nd order LP cut-off frequency (Hz) | 0.3 | 0.7 | |||
2nd order LP damping factor | 0.3 | 0.7 | |||
1st order LP time constant (s) | 0.1 | 0.1 | 0.1 | ||
2nd order HP cut-off frequency (Hz) | 0.5 | 0.5 | 2 | ||
2nd order HP damping factor | 1 | 1 | 1 | ||
1st order HP time constant (s) | 2 | 2 | 2 |
Table 2: Classical Motion cueing algorithm parameters.
Moreover between 65-85s and on the second curvature turn, the high frequent motion was coincided in a higher severity at the vestibular level of the participants for the static platform compared to the dynamic condition (Figure 8). The visuo-vestibular lateral accelerations’ gap reduction in this period originated from the presence of onset cueing for the dynamic platform case.
Table 3 indicated the impact of the motion platform on the reality of the lateral acceleration. Due to this table, 8 out of the 16 subjects had a negative correlation in terms of ‘vestibular lateral accelerationvehicle CG lateral acceleration’ who participated in the condition of static platform. Those negative correlations emphasized the sensory cue conflict (50% of the sensory conflict incidence).
Static condition Correlation of vestibular lateral acceleration-vehicle CG lateral acceleration |
Dynamic condition Correlation of vestibular lateral acceleration-vehicle CG lateral acceleration |
|
---|---|---|
r | r | |
Subject 1 | 0.2526 | 0.5441 |
Subject 2 | -0.5182 | 0.5631 |
Subject 3 | 0.2088 | 0.5622 |
Subject 4 | -0.3482 | -0.3482 |
Subject 5 | -0.3868 | 0.3197 |
Subject 6 | 0.0554 | 0.5775 |
Subject 7 | -0.0638 | 0.2091 |
Subject 8 | -0.2779 | -0.1919 |
Subject 9 | 0.4647 | 0.6512 |
Subject 10 | -0.1670 | 0.2149 |
Subject 11 | -0.0579 | 0.6596 |
Subject 12 | 0.0779 | 0.2555 |
Subject 13 | 0.0743 | 0.6227 |
Subject 14 | 0.1590 | 0.6702 |
Subject 15 | 0.0756 | 0.4458 |
Subject 16 | -0.0157 | 0.2651 |
Subject 17 | 0.5941 | |
Subject 18 | 0.5664 | |
Subject 19 | 0.6036 | |
Subject 20 | 0.6165 | |
Subject 21 | 0.2659 |
Table 3: Effect of inertial condition on lateral dynamics reality.
Due to this table, 8 out of the 16 subjects had a positive correlation (50% of the sensory conflict avoidance incidence) for the static platform. It meant that; apart from this subject, the rest of them yielded a sensory conflict (negative correlation between vestibular lateral acceleration and vehicle CG lateral acceleration) and divergence from the reality of the lateral dynamics represented on the driving simulator.
For the dynamic case, 19 out of the 21 subjects showed positive correlation. It gave an incidence of 90.48% for avoiding the sensory cue conflict based motion sickness. For the dynamic platform situation, there was only 2 people who indicated a negative correlation. It can be interpreted as a visuo-vestibular sensory conflict with an incidence of 9.52%.
The negative correlations between visual and vestibular lateral accelerations in the static case were based on the higher level of the lateral head movements which could be realized to compensate the lack of the motion platform influence. In other words, it allowed the gap to increase between visual and vestibular level lateral accelerations when the vehicle level lateral accelerations were the identical for the boths cases (Figure 8 and table 3).
Figure 9 illustrated the reality of the lateral dynamics depending on the effect of the inertial cues (motion platform) which were given in figure 1. Figure 9 was also a filled contour mapping for the data yielded in table 3.
These graphs were depicted with respect to the Pearson’s correlation coefficients (r). When the correlation coefficient r was near to “1 (red colour)”, it meant that the driving simulation was representing the lateral acceleration more real. In other words, it presented less contradicting visuo-vestibular cues as from lateral acceleration in accordance with sensory cue conflict theory. In contrast; when the r was closer to “-1 (blue colour)”, it indicated that the lateral dynamics was represented less realistically. Therefore, higher values of discrepancy were resulted between the lateral accelerations values measured from vehicle (visual cue) and vestibular cue levels.
Code (**) depicted how figure 9 was created in MATLAB in order to check visuo-vestibular accelerations conflict for the static and dynamic operations of the platform by utilizing the registered data from the SCANeR software.
**contourf ([rvisves1, rvisves2, rvisves3,…, rvisvesn])
N: number of the subjects participated in the experiments.
rvisves1,2,…,N: correlation coefficient between the vestibular and vehicle level lateral accelerations by individual participants.
A multiple linear regression with a confidence interval of 95%, where the input is the lateral acceleration at visual cues (vehicle lateral acceleration) and the output is the lateral acceleration at vestibular cues (sensed lateral acceleration, Equation 1, was modelled for the static and the dynamic platform cases.
Equation 2 gives the linear regression model for the static platform condition.
….. (2)
Equation 3 gives the linear regression model for the dynamic platform condition.
…..(3)
Figure 10 shows the cue conflict comparison of the both cases for the groups of the subjects. Blue dots indicate the real data for the visual and vestibular acceleration cues which were saved from the right ear level of the subjects and the vehicle model driven. Black continuous lines depict the linear regression models. Grey continuous and dashed lines illustrate the confidence interval of 95% for the mean and the observed values respectively.
According to figure 10, it can be seen that the real data were distributed far from the fitted model line (the coefficient of determination R2=0.072) for the static platform. On the other hand, the real data were more closely gathered to the estimated model line (the coefficient of determination R2=0.429) for the dynamic platform. Furthermore, it is resulted that the visual lateral accelerations (realtime vehicle model in SCANeR studio software) are getting closer (the slope of the model is positive: inclined to right hand side) to the vestibular lateral accelerations (real-time XSens record as a module in SCANeR studio software) for the dynamic situation.
Inversely, the visuo-vestibular lateral accelerations are getting far away from each other (the inclination of the model is negative: inclined to left hand side) which mean that the visuo-vestibular cue conflict are increasing.
Lastly, it is yielded that as theUcomputed (2139)>Uexpected (1300.5) and the computed p-value (p<0.0001) is lower than the significance level alpha=0.05, one should reject the null hypothesis H0, and accept the alternative hypothesis Ha after applying the two-tailed Mann-Whitney U test to check the difference significancy between the static and dynamic situations of the vestibular level sensed lateral accelerations where the tested hypotheses were given below:
H0: The difference of location between the samples from the static (N=16) and the dynamic (N=21) cases is equal to 0.
Ha: The difference of location between the samples from the static (N=16) and the dynamic (N=21) cases is different from 0.
In this research, also the perception fidelity was compared for the two conditions. In order to achieve this goal, Pearson’s correlation method was benefited. Three questions were given to the subjects and they were asked to fill in depending on what they perceived during each driving session. The questions regarding the motion sickness (disorientation related) were composed of as the following: Q1- Were you at the point to vomit? Q2- Have you felt nausea? Q3- Have you felt dizziness?
The answers were scaled in equal ten parts as from 1: too little to 10: too strong.
Due to table 4, it can be seen that the ay_vest_sta (vestibular level lateral acceleration at static motion platform) was positively correlated (r=0.143, p=0.318 and r= 0.056, p=0.695 between the sensed lateral acceleration and the propensity to vomit, feeling nausea respectively) with the disorientation related subjective evaluations whereas the ay_ vest_dyn (vestibular level lateral acceleration at dynamic motion platform) was negatively correlated (r=-0.114, p=0.424; r=-0.119, p=0.407 and r=-0.143, p=0.318 between the sensed lateral acceleration and the propensity to vomit, feeling nausea, feeling dizziness respectively) with the subjective perceptions. In general, it shows that the perception proximity to the measured vestibular level lateral accelerations (ay_vest) was coincided for the static case rather than the dynamic one. Because the visuo-vestibular sensorial cue conflict increased at the static hexapod platform compared to the dynamic one (Table 4 and figures 8-10). In particular, merely the dizziness feeling had a significant positive correlation (r=0.293, p=0.037<0.05) with the sensed lateral acceleration at the static condition.
Static motion platform (N=16) | Dynamic motion platform (N=21) | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Variables | Q1s | Q2s | Q3s | Variables | Q1d | Q2d | Q3d |
ɑy_vest_sta | 0.143 (p=0.318) | 0.056 (p=0.695) | 0.293 (p =0.037) | ɑy_vest_dyn | -0.114 (p=0.424) | -0.119 (p=0.407) | -0.143 (p=0.318) |
Table 4: Effect of inertial condition on lateral dynamics reality in absence of Visuo-vestibular restitution.
The reality of the represented lateral dynamics on driving simulators was discussed in this paper. After having completed these experimental phases, due to the Pearson’s correlations, it was proved that the dynamic platform provided a closer lateral dynamics representation between real-time vehicle model (visual cues) and realtime vestibular cues levels. It can be concluded that having dynamic platform represented a higher lateral dynamics reality in terms of data acquisition and measurements. For the dynamic case, there was a positive correlation with an incidence of 90.48% for N=21, whereas a negative correlation with an incidence of 50% for N=16 was yielded for the static one.
The multiple linear regression model resulted as a better fit and a positive slope for the visual-vestibular lateral accelerations for the dynamic (R2=0.429, N=21) platform, whereas it was yielded as a weak fit and a negative slope for the visual-vestibular accelerations for the static (R2=0.072, N=16) platform.
The two-tailed Mann-Whitney U test proved that there was a significant difference between the static and the dynamic cases in terms of the vestibular level lateral accelerations (Ucomputed (2139)>Uexpected (1300.5), p<0.0001).
The perception fidelity illuminated that there was a significant positive correlation (r=0.293, p=0.037<0.05) between the vestibular level accelerations and feeling dizziness for the static platform simulator whereas there was no significant correlation between the vestibular level accelerations and the disorientation perception.
As prospective researches, the reality of having motion cueing algorithms with feedback control will be surveyed in terms of multisensory level dynamics approach (neuromuscular cues (EMGelectromyography), visual cues (vehicle level), vestibular cues (head level), inertial cues (motion platform level)) and with respect to postural stability.