Journal of Psychology & Psychotherapy

Journal of Psychology & Psychotherapy
Open Access

ISSN: 2161-0487

+44 1478 350008

Research Article - (2013) Volume 0, Issue 0

Substance Use Attenuates Physiological Responses Associated With PTSD among Individuals with Co-Morbid PTSD and SUDS

Telsie A Davis1,2, Tanja Jovanovic1*, Seth Davin Norrholm1,2, Ebony M Glover1, Mahogany Swanson1,3, Sarah Spann1 and Bekh Bradley1,2
1Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Emory University School of Medicine, USA, E-mail: telsiedavis@emory.edu
2Mental Health Service, Atlanta VA Medical Center, USA, E-mail: telsiedavis@emory.edu
3Department of Psychology, Georgia State University, USA, E-mail: telsiedavis@emory.edu
*Corresponding Author: Tanja Jovanovic, Assistant Professor, Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences Emory University School of Medicine, 49 Jesse Hill Jr. Dr., Suite 331, Atlanta, GA 30303, USA, Tel: 404-778-1485, Fax: (404) 778- 1488 Email:

Abstract

Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is often conceptualized from a fear conditioning perspective given individuals with PTSD demonstrate a reduced ability to inhibit fear even under safe conditions as compared to those without PTSD. The self-medication hypothesis suggests that individuals with PTSD often develop substance use disorders (SUDs) as an attempt to mitigate trauma-related distressing emotions. This investigation examined this hypothesis in a sample 214 participants, of which 81 did not meet criteria for either PTSD or SUDs (No diagnosis Control group); 33 met criteria for lifetime PTSD, but not SUDs (PTSD only group); 54 met criteria for lifetime SUDs, but not PTSD (SUDs only group); and 46 met lifetime criteria for both disorders (PTSD+SUDs group). PTSD was assessed using the modified PTSD Symptoms Scale (mPSS), and SUDs were assessed using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID). The startle magnitude was assessed using electromyography (EMG) of the eyeblink muscle in response to an acoustic startle probe. Fear-potentiated startle (FPS) was analyzed by comparing startle magnitude at baseline to startle during a fear conditioned stimulus. Results showed that PTSD significantly increased startle responses. However, there was a significant effect of SUDs on fear-potentiated startle to the danger signal, in that those who met criteria for SUDs had reduced fear compared to those who did not. The individuals who had co-morbid PTSD and SUDs did not differ from the Control group. Findings indicate that SUDs may attenuate exaggerated fear responses associated with PTSD. Consistent with the self-medication hypothesis, results suggest that substance use may co-occur with PTSD because it reduces heightened fear load and may allow normalized function in traumatized individuals.

<

Keywords: PTSD, Substance abuse, Hyperarousal, Fear responding, Acoustic startle

Introduction

There is a high degree of co-morbidity with substance use disorders (SUDs) among individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) [1-3]. Among individuals with a lifetime PTSD diagnosis, 51.9% of men and 27.9% of women also met lifetime criteria for alcohol abuse or dependence, and 34.5% of men 26.9% of women also met lifetime criteria for drug abuse or dependence [1]. Conversely, among individuals with a lifetime diagnosis of alcohol abuse, 2.5% of men and 10.5% of women also had a lifetime diagnosis of PTSD [2]. For those with a lifetime diagnosis of alcohol dependence, the prevalence rate of a lifetime diagnosis of PTSD increased to 10.3% of men and 26.2% of women [3].

Individuals with co-morbid PTSD and SUDs are significantly more likely to have poorer treatment compliance, retention, and outcome in comparison to individuals with a single diagnosis of either disorder [4,5]. To address these problems, research studies and randomized controlled trials are being conducted and the numbers of investigations are increasing. However, evidence-based interventions to treat this comorbidity remain few in number and questions of efficacy remain [4]. We suggest that a better understanding of the functional relationship between PTSD and SUDs could yield more nuanced treatment considerations that may ultimately improve treatment outcomes for individuals with this co-morbidity.

Much of the published data concerning the association between PTSD and SUDs is consistent with the widely-held “self-medication hypothesis,” which posits that individuals with PTSD intentionally use substances of abuse to alleviate or manage their trauma-related symptoms [6-8]. Substantial findings support a pathway whereby PTSD develops first [9,10]. The National Comorbidity Survey replication documented that the median age of onset for anxiety disorders is nine years earlier than for SUDs [11]. Additionally, and particularly for women, robust retrospective data show that problematic substance use develops in response to using substances to cope with the negative effects of trauma exposure [12-17]. Support for the self-medication hypothesis is strong, but not universal. As such, other hypotheses remain tenable.

Three other distinct hypotheses on the PTSD-SUDs link are welldocumented in the literature. The “high-risk hypothesis” proposes that individuals with SUDs are at a greater risk of developing PTSD due to increased vulnerability to interpersonal violence (e.g., sexual assault) that ensues from their at-risk, substance-abusing lifestyle [16,18]. The “susceptibility hypothesis” suggests that individuals with SUDs have an increased vulnerability to developing PTSD due to genetic or psychological impairments that result from substance use [18,19]. The “cross-sensitization hypothesis” is derived from animal models of addictive behaviors (for a review see earlier work [20]) that have shown that exposure to stressors enhances psychostimulantinduced dopamine transmission along the mesocorticolimbic pathway and increases the locomotor [21,22] and rewarding [23,24] effects of stimulants such as cocaine. From a clinical perspective, this suggests that stress (i.e., trauma) primes the reward system such that when an individual uses substances of abuse, he or she becomes more susceptible to the rewarding effects of the drug(s), increasing the likelihood of the development of SUDs.

Clarity about the functional association between PTSD and SUDs is necessary to identify treatment targets specific to this co-morbidity. The competing hypotheses to date are likely due, at least in part, to the inability of psychological research strategies to capture direct empirical support [25]. Especially given that the majority of the psychological research is based on self-report data and cross-sectional designs that do not allow for the testing of causation [25]. We suggest that objective, psychophysiological measurement can advance our understanding about the functional association between PTSD and SUDs beyond that previously offered by psychological assessment solely reliant on selfreport and cross-sectional data.

The fear-related symptoms of PTSD (e.g., re-experiencing/intrusion) are often conceptualized from a fear-conditioning perspective given individuals with PTSD have been reported to exhibit impaired fear conditioning in comparison to those without PTSD [26]. This makes it possible to investigate PTSD using physiological measurements such as the acoustic startle response (ASR). Among individuals with PTSD, the degree of fear acquisition and fear expression is so robust that the prefrontal cortex is unable to inhibit exaggerated amygdala signaling [27]. This difficulty inhibiting fear responses is reflected in research on psychophysiological responses of individuals with PTSD, including increased levels of ASR [28].

A growing body of research supports the idea that substance use serves as an anxiety-regulation strategy employed by traumatized individuals to attenuate or control affective distress (e.g., fear and hyperarousal) associated with PTSD [8,25]. This idea is supported by psychophysiological studies in humans and other animal models showing an association of substance use with diminished ASR [29]. For example, chronic cocaine dependence in early remission [30] and opioid maintenance therapy [31] have been associated with significant reductions in ASR. Thus, we propose using psychophysiological measurement to investigate whether co-morbid substance use diminishes the increased ASR associated with PTSD.

The aim of this investigation was to use the acoustic startle paradigm to determine whether substance use functions as an anxiety-regulation strategy on a physiological level among individuals with co-morbid PTSD and SUDs. Based on: (1) past studies demonstrating greater fear-potentiated startle in PTSD subjects during fear acquisition and fear extinction (i.e., fear loading) in comparison to control subjects [32] and (2) robust data in support of the self-medication hypothesis identifying substance use as an emotion-regulation strategy, we hypothesize that individuals with co-morbid PTSD and SUDs will demonstrate a diminished fear response at baseline and conditioning in comparison to individuals with PTSD only, but a greater fear response in comparison to individuals with SUDs only or those without PTSD or SUDs. We also hypothesized that PTSD would be associated with higher fear responses to safety signals, based on our previous work with this disorder [28].

Methods

Participants

The study included 214 participants, of which 81 did not meet criteria for either PTSD or SUDs (Control group), 33 met criteria for lifetime PTSD, but not SUDs (PTSD Only group), 54 met criteria for lifetime SUDs, but not PTSD (SUDs Only group), and 46 met criteria for lifetime diagnoses for both disorders (PTSD+SUDs group). Participants were recruited as part of a larger study examining the contribution of genetic and environmental factors to the development of PTSD among a largely African-American, low socioeconomic status, and inner-city population seeking non-psychiatric services at a large, urban Southeastern U.S. hospital. Exclusion criteria included active psychosis, hearing impairment, positive toxicology on a urine drug screen for cocaine and opiates, along with major medical illnesses as assessed through health and physical examinations conducted by licensed medical professionals. All participants were screened for auditory impairment using an audiometer (Grason-Stadler, Model GS1710) and were required to detect tones at 30 dB (A) SPL at frequencies between 250 to 4000 Hz. Before participation in the study, participants provided written informed consents approved by the Emory University Institutional Review Board and the Grady Health System Research Oversight Committee.

Measures

Psychological measures: In addition to the diagnostic interview, all participants were administered six measures to assess specific psychological symptom presentations. The Trauma Experiences Inventory (TEI) is a 14-item self-report instrument used to assess the occurrence, intensity, and frequency of 13 different types of traumatic events across the life span [33]. It is a measure of both child and adult trauma exposure. The total frequency score was used in this investigation to control for level of lifetime trauma exposure.

The Childhood Trauma Questionnaire–Short Form (CTQ-SF) is a 28-item self-report inventory administered to measure childhood physical, sexual, and emotional abuse [34]. Empirical findings have established that the CTQ-SF has demonstrated good internal reliability and criterion validity and is psychometrically comparable to the original 70-item measure [34]. The CTQ-SF yields a total score, as well as a subscale score for each type of child maltreatment assessed. The total score was used as a covariate in this analysis.

The Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) is a 21-item questionnaire used to detect depressive symptoms and severity of those symptoms over the two-week period prior to assessment [35]. Items are measured on a scale of 0 to 3, and the total score was used as an indicator of depressive symptomatology in this study.

The Modified PTSD Symptoms Scale (mPSS) is a 17-item selfreport scale used to assess PTSD symptom severity over the twoweek period prior to rating [36]. The items on the mPSS mirror the DSM-IV criteria for PTSD. The scale renders both a total score and a total score for each PTSD symptom cluster (i.e., re-experiencing, avoidance/numbness, and hyperarousal). Research indicates the mPSS demonstrates adequate internal consistency and good concurrent validity [36]. The mPSS total score and the total score for each symptom cluster were examined in this study.

The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID) is a clinician-administered, validated assessment measure administered to evaluate the presence or absence of SUDs within our study population [37].

The Kreek–McHugh–Schluger–Kellogg scale (KMSK) is selfreport scale that assesses the amount, frequency, and duration of selfexposure to alcohol, opiates, cocaine, marijuana, and tobacco [38]. Lifetime use of substances as well as current use within the 30 days prior to assessment is measured. Lifetime substance use was examined in this study sample.

Startle response measurements: The eye blink component of the ASR was measured by electromyogram (EMG) recordings of the right orbicularis oculi muscle with two 5 mm disposable Ag/AgCl electrodes filled with electrolyte gel. One electrode was placed 1cm below the pupil of the right eye and the second was positioned 1cm below the lateral canthus. We used a Check trode impedance meter (1089 MKIII, UFI, Morro Bay, CA) to ensure impedance levels were less than 6 kOhm for each participant. The startle probe was a 108 dB (A) SPL, 40msec burst of broadband noise with near instantaneous rise time, delivered binaurally through headphones (Maico, TDH-39-P). Startle response data were acquired using Biopac MP150 for Windows (Biopac Systems, Inc., Aero Camino, CA) and stored on the hard drive of a Windows XP laptop computer. All data were sampled at 1,000 Hz and amplified with an increase of 5,000 using the EMG module of the Biopac system. MindWare software (MindWare Technologies, Ltd., Gahanna, OH) was used to filter, rectify, and smooth the startle response data, as well as export it for statistical analyses. The EMG signal was filtered with low- (28 Hz) and high-frequency (500 Hz) cut-offs, respectively. The maximum amplitude of the eye blink muscle contraction occurred 20- 200 msecs after presentation of the startle probe, and it was used as a measure of the ASR.

The fear-potentiated startle task included two phases: habituation and fear conditioning. The habituation phase consisted of six startle probes presented alone (noise-alone trials, NA). Immediately following habituation, participants underwent the fear conditioning phase, which consisted of three blocks, each of which included four trials of the reinforced conditioned stimulus (CS+, “danger cue”), four trials of the non-reinforced conditioned stimulus (CS-, “safety cue”), and four NA trials for a total of 12 trials per block. All CS+ trials were reinforced with the aversive unconditioned stimulus (US), while the CS- trials were not reinforced. Both CS’s were different colored shapes presented on a computer monitor and were six seconds in duration. The US was a 250 ms air blast with an intensity of 140 psi directed to the larynx. The air blast was emitted by a compressed air tank attached to the polyethylene tubing and controlled by a solenoid switch. This US has been used in our studies previously [39,40] and produces robust fearpotentiated startle. In all phases of the experiment, inter-trial intervals were of randomized duration ranging from 9 to 22 seconds.

Cognitive awareness measurement: A response keypad unit (SuperLab, Cedrus, Corp., San Pedro, CA) was incorporated into the startle session in order to assess trial-by-trial US-expectancy and contingency awareness [41]. Subjects were instructed to respond on each CS trial, within 3 seconds of CS onset, by pressing one of three buttons: a button marked ‘+’ when they expected the US, a second button marked “-” when they did not expect the US, and a third button marked ‘0’ they were uncertain of the contingency. The exact instructions given to the subjects were: “During this experiment you will hear some sudden tones and noises in addition to seeing several colored shapes on the computer monitor. The noises are there to elicit startle and occur every time something happens. However, some of the shapes will be followed by the blast of air while other shapes will not. Throughout the experiment please press the button on the keypad to tell us whether you think a shape will be followed by air (the plus sign), or will not be followed by air (the minus sign). If you do not know, press the 0 sign. You should press a button for each shape.”

Data analysis

The group variables in the analyses were the diagnostic categories for PTSD and SUDs, resulting in two between-groups factors: SUD (2 levels) and PTSD (2 levels). This allowed for the analyses of main effects of each disorder as well as any interaction effects. We also followed up significant effects with Post-hoc comparison between four groups: No diagnosis Control, PTSD only, SUDs only, and co-morbid PTSD+SUDs.

The magnitude of the startle response across the three conditioning blocks and trial types (NA, CS+, CS-) was compared between the two factors using a mixed model 3 (Block)×3 (Trial Type)×2 (SUD)×2 (PTSD) analysis of variance (ANOVA). Contrasts were analyzed for Block (linear trend) and Trial Type (NA vs CS+, CS+ vs. CS-). In addition, in order to compare the degree of fear-potentiated startle between groups while controlling for individual variability in baseline startle responses (i.e., to the NA trials), we calculated a Difference Score by subtracting startle magnitude to the NA trials from the startle magnitude on CS+ trials and CS- trials in each conditioning block. These Difference Scores were also used as dependent variables in a mixed model ANOVA as described above. As mentioned, we compared between-group differences using a four group categorization (Control, PTSD only, SUDs only, and PTSD+SUDs). This categorization was also used for descriptive statistics to better compare the individual groups. Post-hoc group comparisons were performed using Tukey’s HSD tests. All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 20 for Windows. For repeated-measures ANOVA, we used the Huynh-Feldt correction for degrees of freedom. Alpha was set at 0.05.

Results

Demographic data for the four groups are shown in Table 1. As a whole, the average age was 39.97 years old (SD=11.78), primarily African American (92.6%), 66.0% female, and 78.6% were unemployed. There were significant group differences in age, F(3,214)=4.68, p=.003, sex, χ2(3)=24.14, p<.001, race, χ2(12)=20.91, p=0.05, and unemployment, χ2(3)=9.35, p<0.05. As shown in Table 1, the co-morbid PTSD+SUDs group was older than the PTSD Only group, the proportion of females was higher in the Control and PTSD Only groups, and lower in the SUDs Only group. In addition, the proportion of African American was highest in the Control group. Finally, participants in the Control group were less likely to be unemployed than those in the three diagnostic groups. Table 2 shows the clinical data across the four groups. The co-morbid PTSD+SUDs group had greater total PTSD symptoms as compared to the SUDs only and PTSD only group, and higher lifetime cocaine use than the SUDs only and PTSD only groups. Looking at PTSD symptom clusters, the co-morbid group had significantly higher avoidance and hyper-arousal symptoms than all other 3 groups, while the re-experiencing symptoms were comparable to the PTSD only group and higher than the Control and SUDs only groups.

Demographic CONTROL (N=81) PTSD ONLY (N=33) SUD ONLY (N=54) PTSD + SUD (N=46)
Age, M(SD) years 39.5 (12.8) 34.8 (12.5) 40.0 (11.1) 44.5 (8.3)b
Sex, N (%) female 65 (80.2)# 27 (81.8)# 24 (43.6)@ 26 (56.5)
Ethnicity, N (%) African American 80 (98.8)# 28 (84.8) 48 (88.9) 43 (93.5)
Employment, N (%) unemployed 55 (67.9)@ 29 (87.9) 44 (81.5) 40 (87.0)

Abbreviations: PTSD=Posttraumatic stress disorder; SUD=Substance dependence disorder
a=different from CONTROL group; b=different from PTSD ONLY group; c=different from SUD ONLY group
#=greater likelihood; @=lower likelihood

Table 1: Demographic data for the participants in the study across PTSD and SUD groups.

Measure (M,SD) CONTROL PTSD ONLY SUD ONLY PTSD + SUD
Trauma exposure lifetime (TEI) 3.3 (2.8) 5.7 (3.2)a 6.1 (3.1)a 8.0 (3.3)a,b,c
PTSD Symptoms (mPSS) 6.9 (7.5) 23.8 (9.6)a,c 8.5 (6.7) 28.8 (8.2)a,b,c
Re-experiencing Symptoms (mPSS) 1.7 (2.8) 5.9 (3.5)a,c 1.9 (2.3) 7.0 (3.8)a,c
Avoidance Symptoms (mPSS) 2.5 (3.4) 9.8 (4.4)a,b,c 3.5 (3.5) 11.9 (3.7)a,b,c
Hyperarousal Symptoms (mPSS) 2.8 (3.1) 8.2 (3.5)a,b,c 3.2 (3.2) 9.8 (3.3)a,b,c
Lifetime Alcohol Use (KMSK) 6.1 (4.4) 6.8 (4.5) 9.9 (2.8)a 10.3 (3.1)a,b
Lifetime Cocaine Use (KMSK) 0.3 (1.4) 1.0 (3.0) 7.1 (6.8)a 10.4 (6.1)a,b,c
Lifetime Marijuana Use (KMSK) 2.9 (3.9) 4.8 (4.4) 10.2 (4.3)a,b 10.0 (4.4)a,b
Lifetime Heroin Use (KMSK) 0.2 (1.0) 0.1 (0.3) 2.0 (4.1)a,b,d 0.7 (1.6)

Abbreviations: PTSD=Posttraumatic stress disorder; SUD=Substance dependence disorder; TEI=Traumatic experiences inventory; KMSK=Kreek–McHugh– Schluger–Kellogg scale.
a=different from CONTROL group; b=different from PTSD ONLY group; c=different from SUD ONLY group

Table 2: Clinical data for PTSD symptoms and lifetime substance abuse across the PTSD and SUD groups.

A mixed model 3×3×2×2 ANOVA of startle magnitude with Block (3 levels)×Trial Type (3 levels)×SUDs (2 levels)×PTSD (2 levels) as factors resulted in a significant interaction effect of Block×Trial Type×SUDs×PTSD, F(4, 836)=3.67, p=0.006. We followed up the interaction by examining the effect of Trial Type×SUDs×PTSD during the last two blocks of conditioning, when learning is maximal. Figure 1 shows the startle amplitude across Trial Types and diagnoses. We found a significant interaction of SUDs and Trial Type for the NA vs CS+ contrast, F(1, 210)=4.11, p<0.05, and a significant main effect of PTSD, F(1, 210)=4.29, p<0.05, with the PTSD subjects having higher startle responses compared to non-PTSD subjects. Although we did not find an interaction effect of SUDs with PTSD, we wanted to examine fear potentiation (NA vs. CS+), and discrimination (CS+ vs. CS-) in each diagnostic category separately given our hypotheses based on our previous findings in PTSD.

Figure

Figure 1: Mean ± SE startle magnitude during late fear conditioning across Trial Type and diagnostic groups. All groups show increased startle responses to the CS+ compared to the NA trials. Only the Control (neither diagnosis) and the SUD only groups show significantly higher startle to the CS+ than the CS-. **=p<.001.

We compared the three trial types within each group and found that all four groups demonstrated successful fear conditioning with a significant increase in startle response to the CS+ compared to NA (Control, F(1,80)=32.51, p<0.001; PTSD only, F(1,32)=18.55, p<0.001; SUDs only, F(1,53)=22.60, p<0.001; PTSD+SUDs, F(1,45)=20.45, p<0.001). However, only the Control and SUDs only groups showed significantly higher startle response to the CS+ compared to CS- (Control, F(1,80)=20.11, p<0.001; SUDs only, F(1,53)=8.85, p=0.004), while neither of the PTSD groups did, (Figure 1). We repeated the above analysis while co-varying for age and found that the Control group still demonstrated significant fear conditioning to the CS+ and discrimination between the CS+ and CS-. The PTSD Only group showed significant fear conditioning to the CS+, but not discrimination. The last two groups no longer showed significant fear conditioning or discrimination.

In order to compare the degree of fear-potentiated startle between groups and control for individual differences in baseline startle, we calculated a Difference Score by subtracting startle responses to the NA from startle to each CS. We then again examined the Difference Score between CS+ and CS- during late conditioning within each diagnostic group. We again found that the Control and SUDs only group showed significant discrimination (F(1,80)=20.12, p<0.001, and F(1,53)=8.85, p<0.005, respectively), while the PTSD only and PTSD+ SUDs groups did not (Figure 2). However, after co-varying for age, the SUDs only group no longer showed significant discrimination. Given our hypotheses regarding safety signals, we examined the responses to danger cues and safety cues separately, and used each Difference Score as the dependent variable in a 2×2 ANOVA with SUDs (2 levels) and PTSD (2 levels) as between-groups variables. As shown in Figure 2, there was a main effect of SUDs on the fear-potentiated startle to the CS+, F(1,210)=5.74, p=0.02), in that those with substance disorders had reduced fear responses in both PTSD groups. With regard to the CS-, we found a main effect of PTSD, F(1,210)=6.13, p=0.01), in that those who met criteria for the diagnosis showed increased fear to the safety signal across both SUDs groups. We again repeated the analysis with age as the co-variate and the effects remained significant. Therefore, the comorbid SUDs+PTSD group had reduced levels of fear to the danger cue and increased fear to the safety cue, resulting in poor discrimination between the two stimuli.

Figure

Figure 2: Mean ± SE difference score (calculated as the difference in startle magnitude between the CS and NA) during late fear conditioning across Trial Type and diagnostic groups. Only the Control (neither diagnosis) and the SUDs only groups show significantly higher startle to the CS+ than the CS-. The two SUDs groups have lower fear-potentiated startle to the CS+, and the two PTSD groups have higher startle to the CS-. **=p<.001; ¥=p<.05 main effect of SUD; #=p<.05 main effect of PTSD.

In order to assess whether SUDs had an impact on cognitive awareness of the reinforcement contingencies in the fear conditioning task, we examined the data from the trial-by-trial US-expectancy responses for each CS. These data were available for a subset of participants, n=147 (Control, n=53; PTSD only, n=17; SUDs only, n=40; PTSD+SUDs, n=37) and were used to categorize participants as aware of the contingencies if they had an expectancy of the US on the CS+ trials, but no expectancy of the US on the CS- trials during the last conditioning block. This resulted in 108 aware participants (73%), equally distributed across the four groups, χ2 = 4.21, p=0.24. We then used the US-expectancy in the aware participants as the dependent variable in a mixed model 3×2×2×2 ANOVA with Block×Trial Type× SUDs×PTSD as the factors, as described above. We found a significant two-way interaction of Block×Trial Type, F(2,204)=70.74, p<0.001, but no main or interaction effects of either SUDs or PTSD. Looking only at late blocks of conditioning, there was a significant effect of Trial Type, F(1,104)=355.77, p<0.0001, with higher US-expectancy on the CS+ than the CS-, (Figure 3). Again there was no significant main effect or interaction effect with SUDs or PTSD.

Figure

Figure 3: Mean ± SE US expectancy reported on the keypad during late fear conditioning across Trial Type and diagnostic groups. All groups showed significantly higher expectancy of the airblast on the CS+ the CS- trials. *** =p<.0001.

Finally, in order to see whether awareness of the experimental contingencies affected the startle results reported above, we compared the startle difference score for CS+ vs. CS- within each group, after removing unaware participants. The results remained the same. In other words, the Control and SUDs only groups showed significant discrimination between the CS+ and CS- on the fear-potentiated startle measures, while the two PTSD groups did not, even when they were cognitively aware of the contingencies. We also repeated the analyses of startle and response pad data after removing individuals that met current criteria (last 30 days) for SUDs on the SCID, N=15. The results did not change, indicating that chronic lifetime drug use was driving the observed SUDs results.

Discussion

The purpose of the present study was to investigate, for the first time to our knowledge, whether an objective, psychophysiological measurement could provide information on the relationship between PTSD and SUDs that would shed light on the reason for their frequent co-occurrence. The findings resulted in two main results. First, we replicated our previous findings of heightened fear-potentiated startle in PTSD subjects as well as a lack of discrimination between danger and safety cues [28]. Second, we found that participants with SUDs had decreased levels of fear-potentiated startle to the danger cue (CS+) compared to the participants without SUDs, in the control and the PTSD only group. This result suggests that substance use attenuates exaggerated fear responding associated with PTSD.

Consistent with the self-medication hypothesis, our results suggest that substance use, in general, may co-occur with PTSD because it reduces heightened fear and allows for normalized function in traumatized individuals. Our previous studies with PTSD indicated that heightened fear responses, termed “fear load”, is associated with slower fear extinction [32] and increased attention bias to negative emotion [42]. High fear load may be one of the aspects of PTSD that results in significant dysfunction and maintenance of symptoms; and it is possible that substance use allows for a self-imposed reduction in fear load resulting in normalized function. It is important to note that the PTSD+SUDs group reported higher levels of clinical symptoms (PTSD total and avoidance and hyperarousal symptom clusters) than the PTSD only group; therefore, the attenuation appears to only alleviate physiological responses rather than self-report of symptoms.

As mentioned above, the ability to discriminate between danger (CS+) and safety (CS-) signals at the physiological level was found in both the Control and SUDs only groups, but not in either PTSD group. The PTSD only group showed high levels of fear on both cues, while the PTSD+SUDs co-morbid group showed less fear to both cues. It is possible that patients who suffer from co-morbid PTSD and SUDs are less fearful, but still unable to discriminate between danger and safety at the physiological level, increasing their likelihood of engaging in high-risk behaviors that predispose them to further trauma exposure. Although co-varying for age did not change the results in the PTSD groups, the SUDs only group no longer showed discrimination between danger and safety; therefore, it is possible that the impact of age on discrimination may explain the deficits in the comorbid group which was on average older than the other groups.

Importantly, results show that the lack of discrimination between cues is not due to cognitive impairments–even chronic substance use did not result in either higher rates of unaware participants or impaired cognitive learning. Furthermore, neither the PTSD only nor the PTSD+SUDs groups showed deficits in awareness or learning (Figure 3). Therefore, the discrimination deficits in these two groups were only associated with psychophysiological responses. These results indicate that there was a discrepancy in the cognitive and psychophysiological responses in the PTSD participants: even though they did not expect the US to be delivered on the CS-trials (Figure 3), they did not show lower levels of fear-potentiated startle compared to the CS+ (Figure 1 and 2). We have observed this dissociation between cognition and fearpotentiated startle responses in several previous studies with PTSD [26,28,32,43]. This dissociation may be due to deficient prefrontal cortex inhibition of amygdala activity in PTSD, which has been shown in neuroimaging studies [44].

Chronic cocaine dependence has been shown to have long-term effects on startle responses, in that cocaine users showed reduced startle even after a year of abstinence [30]. Acute cocaine use is associated with increased activity of the neurotransmitter dopamine (DA) in several brain areas associated with reward [45]. However, chronic cocaine use may result in adaptations in these brain areas, so that the longterm effects of cocaine are associated with lower levels of DA [46]. This neurotransmitter is also known to have effects on startle response by modulating the startle circuit via the amygdala and ventral striatum [47]. Our data support this hypothesis given that we found a main effect of SUDs on fear-potentiated startle to the CS+; it is possible that the hypodopaminergic state in the co-morbid group is the underlying neurobiological mechanism for the reduction in startle responses. On the other hand, although the SUDs groups in this sample had high rates of cocaine abuse, these participants also used significantly higher rates of alcohol, marijuana, and heroin (Table 2). It is possible that the effects are reflective of polysubstance abuse, rather than a single drug action. Future studies should investigate cocaine addiction without comorbid substances in order to determine the effects of cocaine alone. The value of the current study is that it is likely representative of an inner-city, low-income, and largely ethnic minority population in which high rates of trauma exposure and substance use are frequently observed [48].

Limitations and Future Directions

Despite the strength of this investigation, results must be considered in light of study limitations. Measurements of all clinical variables were based on client self-report, which can introduce various biases such as social desirability, omission of data, and retrospective bias. Though use of self-report is an accepted means for assessing psychological factors, future studies that employ longitudinal designs are indicated as participant memories could be selective, inconsistent, or otherwise less unreliable [49]. The reading of instruments to subjects by study interviewers instead of receiving written answers from subjects could have also introduced additional response biases [50].

Relying on participant report to diagnosis lifetime SUDs poses another study limitation. Although a urine test was administered prior to startle testing and those with positive results were excluded from the study, we do not have reliable information on the duration of abstinence or recent usage by study participants. In addition, research that indicates that acute versus chronic substance use has differential effects on DA levels and other transmitter systems in the brain [51] suggests that startle response could be influenced by whether participants actually have current SUDs.

Acknowledgements

We thank the Grady Trauma Project coordinators, Allen W. Graham and Angelo Brown, and research staff, as well as the nurses in the Clinical Research Network of Grady Health Systems. This work was supported by funding from the National Institutes of Health (MH071537, MH092576, MH098212), Howard Hughes Medical Institute, and the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number UL1TR000454.

References

  1. Kessler RC, Sonnega A, Bromet E, Hughes M, Nelson CB (1995) Posttraumatic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch Gen Psychiatry 52: 1048-1060.
  2. Kessler RC, Crum RM, Warner LA, Nelson CB, Schulenberg J, et al. (1997) Lifetime co-occurrence of DSM-III-R alcohol abuse and dependence with other psychiatric disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch Gen Psychiatry 54: 313-321.
  3. Ouimette P, Goodwin E, Brown PJ (2006) Health and well being of substance use disorder patients with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Addict Behav 31: 1415-1423.
  4. Conrod PJ, stewart SH (2005) A critical look at dual-focused cognitive-behavioral treatments for co-mrobid substance use and psychiatric disorders: Strengths, limitations, and future directions. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy: An International Quarterly 19: 261-284.
  5. Najavits LM, Weiss RD, Shaw SR (1997) The link between substance abuse and posttraumatic stress disorder in women. A research review. Am J Addict 6: 273-283.
  6. Khantzian EJ (1997) The self-medication hypothesis of substance use disorders: a reconsideration and recent applications. Harv Rev Psychiatry 4: 231-244.
  7. Quitkin FM, Rifkin A, Kaplan J, Klein DF (1972) Phobic anxiety syndrome complicated by drug dependence and addiction. A treatable form of drug abuse. Arch Gen Psychiatry 27: 159-162.
  8. Stewart SH, Pihl RO, Conrod PJ, Dongier M (1998) Functional associations among trauma, PTSD, and substance-related disorders. Addict Behav 23: 797-812.
  9. Jacobsen LK, Southwick SM, Kosten TR (2001) Substance use disorders in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder: a review of the literature. Am J Psychiatry 158: 1184-1190.
  10. Taylor SE, Lerner JS, Sage RM, Lehman BJ, Seeman TE (2004) Early environment, emotions, responses to stress, and health. J Pers 72: 1365-1393.
  11. Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, Jin R, Merikangas KR, et al. (2005) Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch Gen Psychiatry 62: 593-602.
  12. Campbell-Sills L, Stein MB (2007) Psychometric analysis and refinement of the Connor-davidson Resilience Scale (CD-RISC): Validation of a 10-item measure of resilience. J Trauma Stress 20: 1019-1028.
  13. Cornelis MC, Glymour MM, Chang SC, Tchetgen EJ, Liang L, et al. (2012) Oxytocin receptor (OXTR) is not associated with optimism in the Nurses' Health Study. Mol Psychiatry 17: 1157-1159.
  14. DeFife JA, Drill R, Nakash O, Westen D (2010) Agreement between clinician and patient ratings of adaptive functioning and developmental history. Am J Psychiatry 167: 1472-1478.
  15. Miranda R Jr, Meyerson LA, Long PJ, Marx BP, Simpson SM (2002) Sexual assault and alcohol use: exploring the self-medication hypothesis. Violence Vict 17: 205-217.
  16. Stewart SH, Israeli AL (2002) Substance abuse and co-occurring psychiatric disorders in victims of intimate violence. In Wekerle C, Vall A (Eds.), The violence and addiction equation: Theoretical and clinical issues in substance abuse and relationship violence. Brunner-Routledge, New York, 97-122.
  17. Taylor SE, Karlamangla AS, Friedman EM, Seeman TE (2011) Early environment affects neuroendocrine regulation in adulthood. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 6: 244-251.
  18. Breslau N, Davis GC, Schultz LR (2003) Posttraumatic stress disorder and the incidence of nicotine, alcohol, and other drug disorders in persons who have experienced trauma. Arch Gen Psychiatry 60: 289-294.
  19. Schwartz AC, Bradley RL, Sexton M, Sherry A, Ressler KJ (2005) Posttraumatic stress disorder among African Americans in an inner city mental health clinic. Psychiatr Serv 56: 212-215.
  20. O'Doherty F (1991) Is drug use a response to stress? Drug and Alcohol Dependence 29: 97-106.
  21. Lu L, Shepard JD, Hall FS, Shaham Y (2003) Effect of environmental stressors on opiate and psychostimulant reinforcement, reinstatement and discrimination in rats: a review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 27: 457-491.
  22. Miczek KA, Yap JJ, Covington HE 3rd (2008) Social stress, therapeutics and drug abuse: preclinical models of escalated and depressed intake. Pharmacol Ther 120: 102-128.
  23. Capriles N, Rodaros D, Sorge RE, Stewart J (2003) A role for the prefrontal cortex in stress- and cocaine-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 168: 66-74.
  24. Vezina P, Lorrain DS, Arnold GM, Austin JD, Suto N (2002) Sensitization of midbrain dopamine neuron reactivity promotes the pursuit of amphetamine. J Neurosci 22: 4654-4662.
  25. Swendsen JD, Tennen H, Carney MA, Affleck G, Willard A, et al. (2000) Mood and alcohol consumption: an experience sampling test of the self-medication hypothesis. J Abnorm Psychol 109: 198-204.
  26. Jovanovic T, Norrholm SD, Fennell JE, Keyes M, Fiallos AM, et al. (2009) Posttraumatic stress disorder may be associated with impaired fear inhibition: relation to symptom severity. Psychiatry Res 167: 151-160.
  27. Rauch SL, Shin LM, Phelps EA (2006) Neurocircuitry models of posttraumatic stress disorder and extinction: human neuroimaging research--past, present, and future. Biol Psychiatry 60: 376-382.
  28. Jovanovic T, Norrholm SD, Blanding NQ, Davis M, Duncan E, et al. (2010) Impaired fear inhibition is a biomarker of PTSD but not depression. Depress Anxiety 27: 244-251.
  29. Schellekens AF, Mulders PC, Ellenbroek B, de Jong CA, Buitelaar JK, et al. (2012) Early-onset alcohol dependence increases the acoustic startle reflex. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 36: 574-582.
  30. Corcoran S, Norrholm SD, Cuthbert B, Sternberg M, Hollis J, et al. (2011) Acoustic startle reduction in cocaine dependence persists for 1 year of abstinence. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 215: 93-103.
  31. Walter M, Wiesbeck GA, Degen B, Albrich J, Oppel M, et al. (2011) Heroin reduces startle and cortisol response in opioid-maintained heroin-dependent patients. Addict Biol 16: 145-151.
  32. Norrholm SD, Jovanovic T, Olin IW, Sands LA, Karapanou I, et al. (2011) Fear extinction in traumatized civilians with posttraumatic stress disorder: relation to symptom severity. Biol Psychiatry 69: 556-563.
  33. Sprang G (1997) The traumatic experiences inventory (TEI): A test of psychometric properties. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment 19: 257-271.
  34. Bernstein DP, Stein JA, Newcomb MD, Walker E, Pogge D, et al. (2003) Development and validation of a brief screening version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Child Abuse Negl 27: 169-190.
  35. Beck AT, Steer RA, Ball R, Ranieri W (1996) Comparison of Beck Depression Inventories -IA and -II in psychiatric outpatients. J Pers Assess 67: 588-597.
  36. Foa EB, Riggs DS, Dancu CV, Rothbaum BO (1993) Reliability and validity of a brief instrument for assessing post-traumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress 6: 459-473
  37. First MB, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Gibbons M (1995) Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV-patient edition (SCID-P). American Psychiatric Press, Washington, DC.
  38. Kellogg SH, McHugh PF, Bell K, Schluger JH, Schluger RP, et al. (2003) The Kreek-McHugh-Schluger-Kellogg scale: a new, rapid method for quantifying substance abuse and its possible applications. Drug Alcohol Depend 69: 137-150.
  39. Jovanovic T, Keyes M, Fiallos A, Myers KM, Davis M, et al. (2005) Fear potentiation and fear inhibition in a human fear-potentiated startle paradigm. Biol Psychiatry 57: 1559-1564.
  40. Norrholm SD, Jovanovic T, Vervliet B, Myers KM, Davis M, et al. (2006) Conditioned fear extinction and reinstatement in a human fear-potentiated startle paradigm. Learn Mem 13: 681-685.
  41. Jovanovic T, Norrholm SD, Keyes M, Fiallos A, Jovanovic S, et al. (2006) Contingency awareness and fear inhibition in a human fear-potentiated startle paradigm. Behav Neurosci 120: 995-1004.
  42. Fani N, Tone EB, Phifer J, Norrholm SD, Bradley B, et al. (2012) Attention bias toward threat is associated with exaggerated fear expression and impaired extinction in PTSD. Psychol Med 42: 533-543.
  43. Jovanovic T, Norrholm SD (2011) Neural mechanisms of impaired fear inhibition in posttraumatic stress disorder. Front Behav Neurosci 5: 44.
  44. Shin LM, Rauch SL, Pitman RK (2006) Amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex, and hippocampal function in PTSD. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1071: 67-79.
  45. Koob GF, Bloom FE (1988) Cellular and molecular mechanisms of drug dependence. Science 242: 715-723.
  46. Kuhar MJ, Pilotte NS (1996) Neurochemical changes in cocaine withdrawal. Trends Pharmacol Sci 17: 260-264.
  47. Davis M, Gendelman DS, Tischler MD, Gendelman PM (1982) A primary acoustic startle circuit: lesion and stimulation studies. J Neurosci 2: 791-805.
  48. Alim TN, Graves E, Mellman TA, Aigbogun N, Gray E, et al. (2006) Trauma exposure, posttraumatic stress disorder and depression in an African-American primary care population. J Natl Med Assoc 98: 1630-1636.
  49. Brady KT, Sonne SC (1999) The role of stress in alcohol use, alcoholism treatment, and relapse. Alcohol Res Health 23: 263-271.
  50. Gillespie CF, Bradley B, Mercer K, Smith AK, Conneely K, et al. (2009) Trauma exposure and stress-related disorders in inner city primary care patients. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 31: 505-514.
  51. Brady KT, Sinha R (2005) Co-occurring mental and substance use disorders: the neurobiological effects of chronic stress. Am J Psychiatry 162: 1483-1493.
Citation: Davis TA, Jovanovic T, Norrholm SD, Glover EM, Swanson M, et al. (2013) Substance Use Attenuates Physiological Responses Associated With PTSD among Individuals with Co-Morbid PTSD and Suds. J Psychol Psychother S7:006.

Copyright: © 2013 Davis TA et al., This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Top