Journal of Biomedical Engineering and Medical Devices

Journal of Biomedical Engineering and Medical Devices
Open Access

ISSN: 2475-7586

Research Article - (2016) Volume 1, Issue 2

Visually Guided Horizontal Saccades under the Double-Step Paradigm

Xiu Zhai and John D Enderle*
Department of Biomedical Engineering, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT 06269, USA
*Corresponding Author: John D Enderle, Department of Biomedical Engineering, University of Connecticut, 260 Glenbrook Road, Storrs, Connecticut 06269-3247, USA, Tel: +18604865521 Email:

Abstract

Visually goal-oriented saccades were recorded under the double-step paradigm. Data were analyzed to produce parameter estimates using the system identification technique for a 3rd-order linear horizontal saccadic eye movement model. Statistical analysis of a large human saccade data set provided reliable conclusions of the response properties. Saccade amplitude, latency and inter-saccade interval were discussed with time delay, indicating the parallel programming mechanism, which two saccades to different targets could be programmed simultaneously. The results of neural input estimations suggested that the double-step visual targets may affect the synchronous firing of the saccade responsible neurons in the superior colliculus. 

Keywords: Saccadic eye movement; Double-step stimulus; Response pattern; Latent period; Amplitude; Inter-saccade interval; Oculomotor plant; Agonist neural input; Saccade programming

Introduction

Saccades are the fastest eye movements that enable us to rapidly redirect our line of sight from one target to another one. They are conjugate and ballistic, with a typical duration of 30-100 ms and a latency of 100-300 ms when triggered by visual stimuli. The latency is thought to be the time interval during which the CNS determines whether to make a saccade, and, if so, calculates the distance the eyeball is to be moved, transforming retinal errors into transient muscle activity. Cognitive control and decision-making take place during this preparation period. How will saccades be modified in “mid-flight” by factors that are applied before the eye starts to move? Westheimer first developed the double-step paradigm by presenting subjects with two targets along a horizontal plane with a varying interstimulus interval separating the two targets [1]. Previous studies indicate the “parallel programing” mechanism, which the second saccade programming can take place while the first saccade is being programmed and executed [2-4]. The visual information can be acquired continuously to modify saccade amplitude until about 70-100 ms prior to the eye movement onset, and the CNS appears able to overlap some presaccadic planning [5-8]. Saccade sequence planning is always discussed by analyzing response localization, latency, and intersaccade interval under the double-step condition [2-11].

In addition to the investigation of saccade characteristics, many studies focus on modeling eye plant dynamics in response to saccade controllers. The work provided in this paper is based on a linear 3rdorder model of the oculomotor plant for horizontal saccades by Enderle and coworkers [12-14], while the neural inputs are described by pulse-slide-step waveforms with a post inhibitory rebound burst (PIRB) based on a time-optimal controller [12,13,15-18]. The oculomotor plant consists of the eyeball, a passive Voigt element, and two extraocular muscles, and it has accurate nonlinear force-velocity and length-tension relationships that agree with the data [12,13,17,18]. The inputs to the muscles are the agonist and antagonist active-state tensions, which are derived from a low-pass filtering of the saccadic neural innervation signals. In agreement with the time-optimal controller, previous studies have presented the neural input properties for goal-oriented saccades that trigged by one-step (from primary position to a peripheral position) visual or auditory stimuli [12,19]. Agonist pulse magnitude shows a linear increase in small saccades, since the number of firing neurons determines the pulse size. Agonist pulse magnitude remains approximately constant for larger saccades over 7 degrees, indicating all neurons fire maximally, and the pulse duration increases as a function of saccade amplitude. Here, our interest is centered on the neural inputs to the oculomotor plant under the double-step paradigm. The study described here expands on our previous work [19] and further supports the muscle models as well as the theory of the time-optimal controller under physiological constraints.

Methods

Subjects

Four subjects (two males and two females, aged 20-30) participated this study. All demonstrated normal or corrected-to-normal visions, and none of them disclosed any history of visual or vestibular disorders. The subjects were trained on tasks of regular goal-oriented saccade experiments (one-step) before performing the double-step saccades.

Apparatus

Subjects were seated 830 mm in front of a computer monitor with head stabilized by a high speed eye tracking device (1250 Hz sample rate, by SensoMotoric Instruments (SMI)). The visual targets consisted of white solid dots (4 mm diameter) on a gray background and were displayed on the screen. Seven target positions were presented to the subject at angles of 5°, 10° and 20° from the center (0°) to left or right in the horizontal plane.

The experiments were conducted using custom written software, while data were collected using the eye tracker implemented in the iView X system by SMI. All the tests were performed in an independent, quiet room with normal illumination.

Experiment design

The subjects were instructed to maintain their eyes on the visual target by moving their eyes as fast as possible. A saccade was made when the active target was switched off and another target was switched on. There were three test sessions with the first peripheral target’s duration (S1, shown in Figure 1(a)) set as 70, 140 and 210 ms. The target stepped twice centrifugally in the same direction. Each session included multiple trials, following 0°-5°-10° or 0°-10°-20° double-step displacements, to right or left. The duration of any center or second peripheral stimulus in a trial was random from 3000 to 5000 ms.

biomedical-engineering-medical-Schematic-sequence-target

Figure 1: Schematic representation of the sequence of target events (a), and saccadic responses: (b) double-step and (c) single-step. A is saccade amplitude, L is latency, and Dur is saccade duration. D is delay, which is defined as the time between the end of first peripheral target presentation and the onset of initial saccade (L1- S1). Int is the inter-saccade interval in response.

Data processing and analysis

Raw data contained pupil and gaze information were generated automatically by the iView X system. Gaze data from the left eye were recorded in pixels, and then converted to degrees. The converted eye position data were first plotted for quality and response pattern examination. If two steps were obtained, they were separated for further analysis.

Acceptable saccades data were run in a FORTRAN program which was used to compute parameter estimates for the model of horizontal saccadic eye movements [12,13]. Graphs in Figure 2 are based on parameter estimates calculated by the system identification technique for a double-step response, where the first step (Figure 2 (a)) and second step (Figure 2 (b)) are analyzed separately. The estimated agonist and antagonist neural inputs, Nag and Nant, and active-state tensions, Fag and Fant, are presented in the first two rows. As shown in the bottom rows, the model predictions match amplitude data, datacalculated velocity (derivative of the amplitude) and data-calculated acceleration (second derivative of the amplitude) very well. The accuracy demonstrated in this graph is consistent with the other saccades analyzed. 378 trials of saccade data that contained either double-step (Figure 1(b)) or single-step (Figure 1(c)) responses were successfully analyzed.

biomedical-engineering-medical-Model-neural-Nag

Figure 2: Model estimated neural inputs Nag and Nant (red line), active-state tensions Fag and Fant (blue line) of three saccades. Also shown are the model predictions using the parameter estimates from the system identification technique for amplitude, velocity, acceleration (blue line), and the experimental data (red line). (a): the first step of a 0°-5°-10° response with a glissade. (b): the second step of a same trial.

(Note: “double-step” and “single-step” are both saccade responses under the double-step stimulus paradigm. “One-step” refers to the response when only one peripheral target is presented.)

Results

Saccade response pattern, accuracy and amplitude

Saccade response patterns are related with the duration of the first peripheral target (also defined as inter-stimulus interval, S1). Figure 3 shows the occurrence rate of a double-step response under two trial patterns. When inter-stimulus interval S1 is relatively small (70 ms), subjects are more likely to follow the second target directly without landing on the first peripheral target (shown as a single-step response), resulting a smaller probability of double-step response. As the inter- stimulus interval increases, double-step responses are obtained in almost all tests. It is also shown that double-step responses have a higher chance to appear in 0°-10°-20° trials.

biomedical-engineering-medical-Probability-double-step

Figure 3: Probability of observing double-step responses with different inter-stimulus interval in 0°-5°-10° and 0°-10°-20° experiments.

Under the double-step paradigm, the initial/primary saccade is defined as the first step of a double-step response, or the single-step response. In order to determine the saccade accuracy, initial/primary saccade amplitude is compared with the desired target amplitude. As shown in Figure 4(a), saccade amplitude of the singe-step responses (As) has a relatively low variance, while undershoot happens frequently when respond to a +/-20° target (average As=+18.17° and -18.43°). Regarding to the first steps (A1), average saccade amplitudes are +4.96°, -6.12°, +9.77°, -10.21° in response to +/-5°, +/-10° targets, respectively, and they show a slight higher variance (Figure 4(b)). Further analysis includes percent error calculations (Figure 5), following

biomedical-engineering-medical-saccade-amplitude-target

Figure 4: Initial/primary saccade amplitude vs. target location. (a) single-step responses and (b) first steps of double-step responses. Dashed red lines represent response amplitude equals to target amplitude. Signs of amplitude indicate saccades to right (+) or saccades to left (-).

biomedical-engineering-medical-Percent-first-step

Figure 5: Percent error of the first step amplitude. In (a) 0°-5°-10° experiments, 86% of the -5° eye movements have percent error of A1>0, and 60% of the +5° eye movements have percent error of A1<0. In (b) 0°-10°-20° experiments, 59% of the -10° eye movements have percent error of A1>0, and 65% of the +10° eye movements have percent error of A1<0. Data are from all subjects.

where a positive percent error indicates an overshoot (absolute saccade amplitude is larger than absolute target amplitude) and a negative percent error indicates an undershoot (absolute saccade amplitude is smaller than absolute target amplitude). In abduction (-5° and -10° movement for the left eye) double-step saccades, the eye always first lands on a position that between the locations of the two peripheral targets, resulting a first-step overshoot, while in adduction (+5° and +10° movement for the left eye) double-step saccades, there’s a higher chance of performing a first-step undershoot. This is more obvious in 0°-5°-10° experiments, and the saccade accuracy is very high in 0°-10°-20° experiments.

Saccade amplitude transition function (ATF) was introduced by Becker and Jürgens [2], where the initial/primary response amplitude is plotted verses delay D (defined as the time between the end of first peripheral target presentation and the onset of initial saccade). Figure 6 shows the ATF, indicating that for delays less than about 110 ms, subjects make double-step responses (the amplitude of the first steps is plotted). Average saccade amplitude gradually increases in both directions for delays from 110 to 140 ms (in 0°-5°-10° experiments) or 170 ms (in 0°-10°-20° experiments), and it’s between the locations of the first and second targets. Amplitude then stays relatively constant again when delays get even longer, which means that subjects make a single-step response directly to the second target location.

biomedical-engineering-medical-Amplitude-transition-saccade

Figure 6: Amplitude transition functions: saccade amplitude vs. delay in (a) 0°-5°-10° experiments and (b) 0°-10°-20° experiments. Data includes the first steps (A1) of double-step responses, and single-step responses (As) from all subjects.

Saccade latency and inter-saccade interval

Figure 7 presents the saccade latency distribution of double-step responses. It shows that the saccade latency of the second steps is sometimes longer than the latency of initial/primary saccades that of similar amplitude. Average saccade latencies of two representative subjects are listed in Table 1.

biomedical-engineering-medical-Saccade-latency-distribution

Figure 7: Saccade latency distribution of the first and second steps in (a) 0°-5°-10° and (b) 0°-10°-20° experiments. Data are binned into bins with a width of 20 ms, and they are from all subjects.

  Subject 1 Subject 2
0°-5°-10° Pattern L1 (ms) 186.66 183.42
L2 (ms) 246.96 213.31
0°-10°-20° Pattern L1 (ms) 196.21 175.83
L2 (ms) 256.21 234.82

Table 1: Average saccade latency.

Second-step saccade latency L2 of two subjects is plotted against time delay as shown in Figure 8. At delays less than 40-50 ms, L2 is roughly constant for both subjects, while subject 1 has a slight higher value than subject 2. When delays fall in the range from 50 to 150 ms, L2 shows a linear increase, where the approximate slope is 1.

biomedical-engineering-medical-Second-saccade-latency

Figure 8: Second step saccade latency vs. delay in (a) 0°-5°-10° and (b) 0°-10°-20° experiments.

Inter-saccade interval is defined as the time between the onset of the first saccade and the onset of the second saccade in a double-step response (Figure 1). Plots of inter-saccade interval versus time delay D for two representative subjects are shown in Figure 9. Despite the individual variability, inter-saccade interval trends to decrease with time delay with a slope around negative 0.4, while the change is steeper when delays are less than 50 ms.

biomedical-engineering-medical-Inter-saccade-interval

Figure 9: Inter-saccade interval as a function of time delay.

Agonist pulse magnitude

Parameter estimations were calculated by the system identification technique [12]. Agonist pulse magnitude is defined as the peak magnitude of the agonist neural input, as shown in Figure 2 first row (red). The results of the model estimation are shown in Figure 10, including the pulse magnitude of single-step and the two separate steps in double-step responses for two representative subjects. Overall, the agonist pulse magnitude of subject 1 is around 0.5 N higher than that of subject 2, and subject 2’s data fall in the same range as our previous visual goal-oriented one-step saccade experiment data [19]. For both subjects, pulse magnitude increases linearly with small saccade amplitude under 7°, and stay relatively constant when saccade amplitude is larger. Note that for small saccades around 5°, the trend lines for the first and second steps pulse magnitude show obvious difference, while no such difference is illustrated in larger saccades around 10°. This is observed in all subjects’ data. In Figure 10(b), subject 2 shows a relatively high agonist pulse magnitude in second steps of +10° saccades. This is a particular case and it is not obtained in the data of other subjects. Agonist pulse magnitude of single-step and double-step responses falls into a similar range.

biomedical-engineering-medical-Agonist-pulse-magnitude

Figure 10: Agonist pulse magnitude vs. saccade amplitude. (a): data of subject 1; (b): data of subject 2.

Discussion

In previous studies of double-step saccades, target separations (step) were set at large amplitude (12°, 15°, 45°, etc.) in horizontal plane [2,3,20], while others displayed targets in 2-D coordinates [4,5,7,8]. Our experiment design focuses on horizontal saccades with smaller stimulus steps (5° and 10°) following “staircase” pattern, which allows us to compare the characteristics and neural input properties with visual one-step goal-oriented saccades, and thus expands the saccade system investigation. The 3rd-order linear saccade model that supports the data analysis in this study has been fully examined before [12-14,17-19], thus it can be further applied to the current study, providing accurate saccade characteristics and neural input estimations. Statistical analysis of a large human saccade data set provides reliable conclusions of the response properties under the double-step paradigm.

Saccade characteristics and response properties

Saccade response patterns are related with the inter-stimulus interval and delay D, which is the time between the end of the first target presentation and the onset of the initial/primary saccade. Our saccade transition function (ATF) shows single-step responses occur with long delays, while double-step responses happen with shorter delays. This means the retinal error can be immediately updated when a second target is applied, the programming of the preceding saccade ends and the programming of the saccade to the new target starts at once. As the inter-stimulus interval increases, the preceding saccade programming cannot be modified [2,3,5,20]. Since double-step responses present when delay is shorter than around 110 ms, and the average latency of the first steps is 180-200 ms, it indicates the visual information can be acquired continuously to modify saccade amplitude until about 70-90 ms prior to eye movement onset. We also find that it’s more frequent to skip the landing on the first peripheral targets when the two targets are closer to the primary position (center) (Figure 3).

Overall, saccade accuracy is still very high under the visual doublestep paradigm. For the single-step responses caught in 0°-10°-20° experiments, the saccade amplitude is slight less than desired target amplitude. Such undershoot suggests the retinal error is “updated” rather than directly obtained. In double-step responses, a significant high occurrence rate of first-step overshoot is found in abduction eye movements. This probably accounts for the fact that the lateral rectus eye muscle is usually stronger than the medial rectus muscle.

The timing properties of double-step saccades indicate the saccade programming mechanism. Previous studies have suggested the “parallel programming”, which the programming of a second saccade can be initiated prior to the execution of a preceding saccade [2-4,7]. Our findings further support this conclusion. If the two saccades are programmed in paralleled, each step is only responsible to one of the two targets, and the second step should occur in a relatively fixed period after the onset of the second target, regardless of the timing of the initial saccade [7]. Since the fact that saccade latency is usually independent of saccade amplitude and show a relatively great variance, the range of L2 distribution shown in Figure 7 can be accepted as a “fixed area”. Moreover, from the logic of the parallel, independent saccade programming suggests that the inter-saccade interval should be inversely related to the delay with an ideal slope of negative one [2,4,7]. Our data in Figure 9 show an overall slope of around -0.4, while the change is much steeper when delay is less than 50 ms. Shorter inter-saccade intervals with longer delays suggest the programming of two sequential saccades may be overlapped. Constant L2 with short delays less than 50 ms (Figure 8) also supports such parallel programming mechanism. Note that the mean latency of the second steps is generally longer than that of the first steps, and L2 is larger at longer delays. These indicate a reduction in the speed of processing the second saccades.

Neural input estimation

In the saccadic model used here, the overall neural input increases as the amplitude of saccades increases up to 7° due to the increasing number of firing neurons, with all neurons firing maximally. In large saccades, all neurons fire maximally, which results in a relatively constant pulse magnitude [12-14]. Estimated agonist pulse magnitude doesn’t significantly increase with increasing saccade amplitude for saccades larger than about 7°, but there is a more obvious linear increase for small saccades under 7° (Figure 10). For both subjects, the pulse magnitude appears similar for the initial/primary and the secondary saccades of size around 10°. This is true because only the retinal error determines the active neuron population.

A significant finding can be observed in the agonist pulse magnitude of saccades around 5°: with a similar trending slope, the pulse magnitude of the first-step saccades is different (in most cases, lower) from that of the second-step saccades (Figure 10). Smaller saccades have fewer actively firing agonist neurons than larger saccades, thus the firing synchrony of individual neurons has a greater impact on the overall neural input for small saccades, and any lack of synchrony causes the overall agonist input to be smaller [12-14]. Our results indicate that for the saccade responsible neurons in the superior colliculus, a second presented visual target may affect their synchrony of firing. Since the agonist pulse magnitude between the first and the second steps in larger saccades are similar, the number of firing neurons is not affected by the double-step stimulus.

Conclusion

Two sequential visual targets are presented with varying inter-stimulus interval to trigger double-step saccades. Saccade characteristics and neural input properties are estimated using a 3rd-order linear horizontal saccadic eye movement model. The analysis of response patterns with saccade amplitude, latency, and inter-saccade interval provides evidence of parallel programming mechanism. While the programming of a second saccade can be initiated prior to the execution of a preceding saccade, there may be a penalty associated with a concurrently-programmed second saccade, resulting a longer latency (L2). Supported by the estimated agonist neural input, the double-step stimulus doesn’t affect the number of firing neurons, but the synchrony of firing. The study described here expands our previous work and further supports the muscle model as well as the theory of the time-optimal saccade controller under physiological constraints.

References

  1. Westheimer G (1954) Eye movement responses to a horizontally moving visual stimulus. AMA Archives of Ophthalmology 52: 932-941.
  2. Becker W, Jürgens R (1979) An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli. Vision Research 19: 967-983.
  3. Baizer J, Bender D (1989) Comparison of saccadic eye movements in humans and macaques to single-step and double-step target movements. Vision Research 29: 485-495.
  4. Ray S, Schall JD, Murthy A (2004) Programming of double-step saccade sequences: modulation by cognitive control. Vision Research 44: 2707-2718.
  5. Aslin RN, Shea SL (1987) The amplitude and angle of saccades to double-step target displacements. Vision Research 27: 1925-1942.
  6. Shulgovskiy VV, Slavutskaya MV, Lebedeva IS, Karelin SA, Moiseeva VV, et al. (2015) Saccadic responses to consecutive visual stimuli in healthy people and patients with schizophrenia. Human Physiology 41: 372-377.
  7. McPeek RM, Skavenski AA, Nakayama K (2000) Concurrent processing of saccades in visual search. Vision Research 40: 2499-2516.
  8. Sharika KM, Ramakrishnan A, Murthy A (2014) Use of exocentric and egocentric representations in the concurrent planning of sequential saccades. The Journal of Neuroscience 34: 16009-16021.
  9. Zimmermann E, Morrone MC, Burr D (2015) Visual mislocalization during saccade sequences. Experimental Brain Research 233: 577-585.
  10. Heide W, Blankenburg M, Zimmermann E, Kömpf D (1995) Cortical control of double‐step saccades: implications for spatial orientation. Annals of Neurology 38: 739-748.
  11. Van der Stigchel S, Rafal RD, Bultitude JH (2013) Temporal dynamics of error correction in a double step task in patients with a lesion to the lateral intra-parietal cortex. Neuropsychologia 51: 2988-2994.
  12. Enderle JD, Zhou W (2010) Models of horizontal eye movements. Part 2: A 3rd-order linear saccade model. Morgan & Claypool Publishers, San Rafael, CA, USA.
  13. Zhou W, Chen X, Enderle JD (2009) An updated time-optimal 3rdorder linear saccadic eye plant model. International Journal of Neural Systems 19: 309-330.
  14. Enderle JD, Sierra DA (2013) A new linear muscle fiber model for neural control of saccades. International Journal of Neural Systems 23: 1350002.
  15. Enderle JD, Wolfe JW (1987) Time-optimal control of saccadic eye movements. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering 34: 43-55.
  16. Enderle JD (2002) Neural control of saccades. Progress in Brain Research 140: 21-49.
  17. Ghahari A, Enderle JD (2014) A Physiological Neural Controller of a Muscle Fiber Oculomotor Plant in Horizontal Monkey Saccades. ISRN Ophthalmology.
  18. Ghahari A, Enderle JD (2014) A Neuron-Based Time-Optimal Controller of Horizontal Saccadic Eye Movements. International Journal of Neural Systems 24: 1450017.
  19. Zhai X, Che C, Mazzarese BD, Colberg AV, Enderle JD (2016) A neural control model for horizontal visual, auditory and auditory-visual bisensory stimuli elicited saccades. Journal Bioengineering and Biomedical Science S3:004.
  20. Ottes FP, Van Gisbergen JA, Eggermont JJ (1984) Metrics of saccade responses to visual double stimuli: two different modes. Vision Research 24: 1169-1179.
Citation: Zhai X, Enderle JD (2016) Visually Guided Horizontal Saccades under the Double-Step Paradigm. J Biomed Eng Med Devic 1:113.

Copyright: © 2016 Zhai X, et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Top